Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: https://ah.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/111780
題名: 高功能自閉症類群障礙學齡兒童心智理論能力表現之研究
A Study of Theory of Mind in School-Aged Children with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders
作者: 鄧宇捷
Teng, Yu-Chieh
貢獻者: 姜忠信
Chiang, Chung Hsin
鄧宇捷
Teng, Yu-Chieh
關鍵詞: 自閉症
心智理論
辯證內容
高功能自閉症
智能優異
Autism
Theory of mind
Justification
High-functioning autism
Intellectually-gifted
日期: 2017
上傳時間: 10-Aug-2017
摘要: 研究目的:心智理論(Theory of Mind)缺陷在過去幾十年來一直是學界所探究的主要自閉症心理病理議題之一,然而Pellicano(2011)提出自閉症心理病理理論的標準使得該理論受到挑戰。本研究旨在發展適切的心智理論作業,以了解高功能自閉症類群障礙症(Autism Spectrum Disorder, ASD)學齡兒童的心智理論表現。\n方法:本研究招募經生理年齡配對之6歲至12歲的智能優異高功能ASD學齡兒童67名以及一般發展學齡兒童51名,將鳳華(2007)發展之心智理論作業進行修訂,作業內容包括慾望或情境引起的情緒與信念、基本信念、第一階錯誤信念、第二階錯誤信念、諷刺、隱喻、失態等分測驗的評估。受試兒童接受心智理論、智能、語言、自閉症症狀嚴重度、適應行為等評估。\n結果:修訂之心智理論測驗其內部一致性信度、評分者間一致性信度以及建構效度表現尚可,「第一階錯誤信念」分測驗的信效度表現較差。高功能ASD兒童與一般發展兒童在心智理論作業得分上有很大的重疊;智能優異高功能ASD兒童整體表現雖然與一般發展兒童相當,但是較一般發展兒童使用較多錯誤的心智辯證內容;非智能優異高功能ASD兒童則是整體表現皆較一般發展兒童弱。心智理論表現雖能顯著預測部分ASD兒童於實驗情境中的社交、溝通、重複性行為症狀表現,但無法預測家長報告之症狀嚴重度和日常適應行為水準。\n結論:本研究修訂之心智理論作業尚可做為探究學齡心智理論發展之參考工具,唯部分分測驗題項內容仍建議需進一步的修訂。心智理論缺陷雖可預測實驗情境中的部分ASD症狀表現,然而智能和語言發展可能補償ASD兒童的部分心智理論能力,且並非所有ASD兒童皆表現出其缺陷,顯示ASD者的心智理論缺陷仍需要更為細膩的測量與探究。針對以上發現進行討論,並給予未來研究和實務上的建議。
Purpose:The theory of mind (ToM) deficits have been one of the main topics of psychopathology in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in the past few decades, however, some challenges were proposed from Pellicano (2011). The purpose of the study was to develop an optimal ToM task and to explore the performance in the school-aged children with high-functioning ASD on ToM.\nMethod:Sixty-seven 6-12 years old children with HFASD and 51 chronological age matched children with typically development (TD) were recruited. The ToM Task developed by Feng Hua (2007) was modified and the task included subtests of “emotion and belief aroused by eagerness or situation”, ”basic belief”, ”1st-order false belief”, “2nd-order false belief”, “irony”, “metaphor”, “Faux Pas” etc.. Participants received assessments of ToM, intelligence, language, ASD symptom severity, and adaptive behavior.\nResult:The modified ToM Task had fair internal consistency reliability, interrater reliability, and construct validity, but the “1st-order false belief” subtest had poor reliability and validity. There was largely overlapped between HFA children and TD children on ToM task scores. Overall performance in the children with IG-HFA was as good as the children with TD, however, they made more false mental justifications than TD children; Non-IG-HFA children’s overall performance was worse than TD children. ToM performance would partially predict symptom severities of social, communication, repetitive behavior under laboratory context in ASD children, but not symptom severities and daily adaptive behaviors reported by parents.\nConclusion:The modified ToM Task could be a tool for exploring ToM development for school-aged children, however, some items are suggested to be further modified. Though the deficit of Theory of mind can partially predict ASD symptoms under laboratory context, IQ and language development might compensate partial ToM ability in children with ASD. Also, not all children with ASD having ToM deficits indicates that subtler measures and explorations on ToM deficits of ASD are needed. The suggestions for future study and practical application is discussed.
參考文獻: 張正芬(2007)。亞斯柏格資優生之適性教育方案(I)研究成果報告(精簡版)。行政院國家科學委員會專題研究成果報告(編號:NSC 95-2614-S-003-019-),未出版。\n陳心怡(1999)。「簡式」魏氏兒童智力量表之建立研究-四個分測驗之組合。測驗年刊,46(2),13-32。\n陳榮華(1997)。魏氏兒童智力量表第三版(中文版)指導手冊。臺北,中國行為科學社。\n許君翎(2007)。注意力缺陷/過動疾患兒童與高功能自閉症兒童在心智理論能力上的差異(碩士論文)。取自臺灣碩博士論文系統。(系統編號095FJU00071006)。\n曹峰銘、蔡佩君、王加恩、呂信慧(2012)。高功能自閉症學齡兒童的心智理論與口語敘說。中華心理學刊,54(3),365-383。\n鄒小蘭、盧台華(2015)。身心障礙資優學生支援服務系統建構之行動研究。特殊教育研究學刊,40(2),1-29。\n鄒啟蓉(2005)。學齡前幼兒“錯誤信念”發展研究。特殊教育研究學刊,29,25-47。\n黃玉枝、黃桂君、宣崇慧(2009)。修訂學齡兒童語言障礙評量表指導手冊。教育部特殊教育工作小組出版。\n鳳華(2007)。中部地區亞斯伯格症兒童「心智理論」(Theory of Mind)的發展現況及心智理論暨社會技巧訓練對亞斯伯格兒童介入成效之研究成果報告(精簡版)。行政院國家科學委員會專題研究成果報告(編號:NSC-95-2413-H-018-003),未出版。\nAbell, F., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2000). Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cognitive Development, 15(1), 1-16.\nAdachi, T., Koeda, T., Hirabayashi, S., Maeoka, Y., Shiota, M., Wright, E. C., & Wada, A. (2004). The metaphor and sarcasm scenario test: A new instrument to help differentiate high functioning pervasive developmental disorder from attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Brain and Development, 26(5), 301-306.\nAmerican Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical manual of mental disorders –Text Revision (4th ed-TR.). Washinton DC: Author.\nAmerican Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washinton DC: Author.\nApperly, I. A., & Butterfill, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states?. Psychological Review, 116(4), 953.\nApperly, I. A., Samson, D., & Humphreys, G. W. (2005). Domain-specificity and theory of mind: evaluating neuropsychological evidence. Trends in cognitive sciences, 9(12), 572-577.\nAssouline, S. G., Nicpon, M. F., & Dockery, L. (2012). Predicting the academic achievement of gifted students with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 42(9), 1781-1789.\nAstington, J. W., Hughes, C., & Zelazo, P. D. (2013). Theory of mind: Self-reflection and social understanding. In Zelazo, P. D. (Ed.), Oxford Handbook of Developmental Psychology (pp. 398-424). USA, Oxford University Press.\nBaron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child’s theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 285-298.\nBaron-Cohen, S. (1994). How to build a baby that can read minds: Cognitive mechanisms in mindreading. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition, 13, 513-552.\nBaron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.\nBaron-Cohen, S. (2001). Theory of mind and autism: A review. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation: Autism, 23, 169-184.\nBaron‐Cohen, S. (2009). Autism: the empathizing–systemizing (E‐S) theory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156(1), 68-80.\nBaron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”?. Cognition, 21(1), 37-46.\nBaron‐Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1986). Mechanical, behavioural and intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4(2), 113-125.\nBaron-Cohen, S., O`Riordan, M., Stone, V., Jones, R., & Plaisted, K. (1999). Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(5), 407-418. \nBaron‐Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high‐functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241-251.\nBegeer, S., Gevers, C., Clifford, P., Verhoeve, M., Kat, K., Hoddenbach, E., & Boer, F. (2011). Theory of mind training in children with autism: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(8), 997-1006.\nBirch, S. A., & Bloom, P. (2007). The curse of knowledge in reasoning about false beliefs. Psychological Science, 18(5), 382-386.\nBrown, R., Hobson, R. P., Lee, A., & Stevenson, J. (1997). Are There “Autistic‐like” Features in Congenitally Blind Children?. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(6), 693-703.\nCallenmark, B., Kjellin, L., Rönnqvist, L., & Bölte, S. (2014). Explicit versus implicit social cognition testing in autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 18(6), 684-693.\nDahlgren, S. O., & Trillingsgaard, A. (1996). Theory of mind in non‐retarded children with autism and Asperger`s syndrome. A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(6), 759-763.\nFlavell, J. H., Miller, P. H., & Miller, S. (1993). Cognitive development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.\nFrith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.\nGreen, S., Pring, L., & Swettenham, J. (2004). An investigation of first‐order false belief understanding of children with congenital profound visual impairment. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22(1), 1-17.\nHappé, F. G. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters` thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129-154.\nHappé, F. G. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66(3), 843-855.\nHappé, F., Booth, R., Charlton, R., & Hughes, C. (2006). Executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: examining profiles across domains and ages. Brain and Cognition, 61(1), 25-39.\nHappé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5-25.\nHill, E. L. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 26-32.\nKleinman, J., Marciano, P. L., & Ault, R. L. (2001). Advanced theory of mind in high-functioning adults with autism. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 31(1), 29-36.\nKlin, A. (2000). Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome: The social attribution task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(07), 831-846.\nKlin, A., Volkmar, F. R., & Sparrow, S. S. (1992). Autistic social dysfunction: Some limitations of the theory of mind hypothesis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(5), 861-876.\nLai M-C, Lombardo MV, Chakrabarti B, Baron-Cohen S (2013) Subgrouping the Autism “Spectrum": Reflections on DSM-5. PLoS Biol 11(4): e1001544.\nLord, C., & Risi, S. (1998). Frameworks and methods in diagnosing autism spectrum disorders. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 4(2), 90-96.\nLord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook Jr, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., ... & Rutter, M. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205-223.\nLord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659-685.\nMcKinnon, M. C., & Moscovitch, M. (2007). Domain-general contributions to social reasoning: Theory of mind and deontic reasoning re-explored. Cognition, 102(2), 179-218.\nMcLean, R. L., Harrison, A. J., Zimak, E., Joseph, R. M., & Morrow, E. M. (2014). Executive function in probands with autism with average IQ and their unaffected first-degree relatives. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(9), 1001-1009.\nMesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2009). The many faces of the still-face paradigm: a review and meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 29(2), 120-162.\nMeltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 838.\nMiller, C. A. (2001). False belief understanding in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 34(1), 73-86.\nMinter, M., Hobson, R. P., & Bishop, M. (1998). Congenital visual impairment and`theory of mind`. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 183.\nMuris, P., Steerneman, P., Meesters, C., Merckelbach, H., Horselenberg, R., van den Hogen, T., & van Dongen, L. (1999). The TOM test: A new instrument for assessing theory of mind in normal children and children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(1), 67-80.\nNicpon, M. F., Allmon, A., Sieck, B., & Stinson, R. D. (2011). Empirical investigation of twice-exceptionality: Where have we been and where are we going?. Gifted Child Quarterly, 55(1), 3-17.\nNicpon, M. F., Doobay, A. F., & Assouline, S. G. (2010). Parent, teacher, and self perceptions of psychosocial functioning in intellectually gifted children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(8), 1028-1038.\nOnishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs?. Science, 308(5719), 255-258.\nOzonoff, S. (1995). Executive functions in autism. In Schopler, E., & Mesibov, G. B. (Eds.), Learning and cognition in autism (pp. 199-219). New York, NY: Springer.\nOzonoff, S., Iosif, A. M., Baguio, F., Cook, I. C., Hill, M. M., Hutman, T., ... & Steinfeld, M. B. (2010). A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(3), 256-266.\nOzonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Brief report: Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(2), 171-177.\nPellicano, E. (2011) Psychological models of autism: An overview. In Roth, I., & Rezaie, P. (Eds.). Researching the Autism Spectrum: Contemporary Perspectives (pp. 219-265). Cambridge University Press.\nPerner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that…” attribution of second-order beliefs by 5-to 10-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39(3), 437-471.\nPeterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (1995). Deafness, conversation and theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36(3), 459-474.\nPeterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (1999). Representing inner worlds: Theory of mind in autistic, deaf, and normal hearing children. Psychological Science, 10(2), 126-129.\nPeterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Slaughter, V. (2012). The mind behind the message: Advancing theory‐of‐mind scales for typically developing children, and those with deafness, autism, or Asperger syndrome. Child development, 83(2), 469-485.\nPilowsky, T., Yirmiya, N., Arbelle, S., & Mozes, T. (2000). Theory of mind abilities of children with schizophrenia, children with autism, and normally developing children. Schizophrenia Research, 42(2), 145-155.\nPlaisted, K., Swettenham, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Children with autism show local precedence in a divided attention task and global precedence in a selective attention task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(05), 733-742.\nPremack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?. Behavioral and brain sciences, 1(04), 515-526.\nPrior, M., Dahlstrom, B., & Squires, T. L. (1990). Autistic children`s knowledge of thinking and feeling states in other people. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31(4), 587-601.\nRepacholi, B. M., & Gopnik, A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires: evidence from 14-and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 33(1), 12.\nReed, T., & Peterson, C. (1990). A comparative study of autistic subjects` performance at two levels of visual and cognitive perspective taking. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 20(4), 555-567.\nRobinson, E. J., & Apperly, I. (1998). Adolescents’ and adults’ views about the evidential basis for beliefs: Relativism and determinism re‐examined. Developmental Science, 1(2), 279-289.\nRubenstein, L. D., Schelling, N., Wilczynski, S. M., & Hooks, E. N. (2015). Lived experiences of parents of gifted students with Autism Spectrum Disorder: The struggle to find appropriate educational experiences. Gifted Child Quarterly, 59(4), 283-298.\nRumsey, J. M. (1985). Conceptual problem-solving in highly verbal, nonretarded autistic men. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 15(1), 23-36.\nRussell, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of autism. In Swanwick, G. (Ed.), Cognitive deficits in brain disorders (pp. 295-323). The Royal College of Psychiatrists.\nRutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). The social communication questionnaire: Manual. Western Psychological Services.\nSchaafsma, S. M., Pfaff, D. W., Spunt, R. P., & Adolphs, R. (2015). Deconstructing and reconstructing theory of mind. Trends in cognitive sciences, 19(2), 65-72.\nScheeren, A. M., de Rosnay, M., Koot, H. M., & Begeer, S. (2013). Rethinking theory of mind in high‐functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(6), 628-635.\nShah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: A research note. Journal of child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24(4), 613-620.\nShah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design task?. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34(8), 1351-1364.\nSchneider, D., Slaughter, V. P., Bayliss, A. P., & Dux, P. E. (2013). A temporally sustained implicit theory of mind deficit in autism spectrum disorders. Cognition, 129(2), 410-417.\nSparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (1989). The vineland adaptive behavior scales. Major psychological assessment instruments, 2, 199-231.\nSullivan, K., Zaitchik, D., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1994). Preschoolers can attribute second-order beliefs. Developmental Psychology, 30(3), 395.\nTager-Flusberg, H. (2001). A reexamination of the theory of mind hypothesis of autism. In Burack J. A., Charman T., Yirmiya N. & Zelazo P. R. (Eds.), The Development of Autism: Perspectives from Theory and Research (pp. 173-193). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.\nTager-Flusberg, H. (2003). Exploring the relationship between theory of mind and social-communicative functioning in children with autism. In Repacholi B. & Slaughter V. (Eds.), Individual differences in Theory of Mind: Implications for Typical and Atypical Development (pp. 197-212). New York: Psychology Press. \nTager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Evaluating the theory-of-mind hypothesis of autism. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 311-315.\nTomasello, M., & Haberl, K. (2003). Understanding attention: 12-and 18-month-olds know what is new for other persons. Developmental Psychology, 39(5), 906.\nTsai, L. Y. (2013). Asperger’s disorder will be back. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(12), 2914-2942.\nWellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory‐of‐mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523-541.\nWeimer, A. A., Dowds, S. J. P., Fabricius, W. V., Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Suh, G. W. (2017). Development of constructivist theory of mind from middle childhood to early adulthood and its relation to social cognition and behavior. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 154, 28-45.\nWhite, S., Hill, E., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2009). Revisiting the strange stories: revealing mentalizing impairments in autism. Child Development, 80(4), 1097-1117.\nWimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children`s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103-128.\nYirmiya, N., Erel, O., Shaked, M., & Solomonica-Levi, D. (1998). Meta-analyses comparing theory of mind abilities of individuals with autism, individuals with mental retardation, and normally developing individuals. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 283.
描述: 碩士
國立政治大學
心理學系
102752006
資料來源: http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G0102752006
資料類型: thesis
Appears in Collections:學位論文

Files in This Item:
File SizeFormat
200601.pdf1.9 MBAdobe PDF2View/Open
Show full item record

Google ScholarTM

Check


Items in DSpace are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.