Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: https://ah.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/52705
DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.advisor萬依萍zh_TW
dc.contributor.advisorI-Ping Wanen_US
dc.contributor.author蕭裕台zh_TW
dc.creator蕭裕台zh_TW
dc.date2011en_US
dc.date.accessioned2012-04-17T01:05:33Z-
dc.date.available2012-04-17T01:05:33Z-
dc.date.issued2012-04-17T01:05:33Z-
dc.identifierG0097555009en_US
dc.identifier.urihttp://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/52705-
dc.description碩士zh_TW
dc.description國立政治大學zh_TW
dc.description語言學研究所zh_TW
dc.description97555009zh_TW
dc.description100zh_TW
dc.description.abstract長久以來,序列式詞彙處理模式以及互動式詞彙處理模式經常爭論孰為合理的語誤生成模型。這份研究企圖利用心理學實證角度探究此議題。原屬於心理學範疇的史楚普技術(1935)或可提供語言學多方面的視角,尤其在我們處理顏色詞彙時詞彙譯碼歷程上與視覺表徵之間的關係。因此我們試圖將此經典心理學技術轉化成宜於語言學研究的語誤誘發實驗。藉由控制變項間的語音近似度,創制了四項實驗,分別是:顏色唸名實驗、顏色誦讀實驗、史楚普唸名實驗、同音詞唸名實驗。結果顯示,即便受試者對語音近似度並未在單項實驗中產生語誤數量與反應時間的差異,但它在不同實驗任務之間卻誘使受試者產生不一樣的反應結果,包括數量上及時間上的顯著差異。\n 此外,我們針對語誤,分析其與目標字之間的關係,研判部分音韻效應是否對詞彙處理網絡造成顯著影響。結果顯示,史楚普效應、音韻結構、音法限制以及聲調效應在語誤數量中均出現顯著的生成量;音節首、韻部以及母音則並未出現顯著效應。史楚普技術除了在語誤證據中為文字閱讀提供自動化效應的解釋外,也讓我們看到音韻效應在不同視覺任務指派中產生顯著差異的結果。如此看來,互動式模型提供了較為簡潔的解釋。來自二元視覺刺激(視覺色彩與顏色詞)的詞彙競爭,該框架可以合理提供理論基礎,並解釋不同視覺任務之間音韻關係依存度的不同。\n 另一方面,我們亦援引同樣技術設計出以音韻單位為導向的唸名實驗,討論語言輸出前規劃單位之議題。我們發現,僅含聲調的音節以及無聲調的音節可在詞彙網絡中做為語言規劃之心理處理單位,促進詞彙處理效能。其他單位如音節首、介音、母音、韻尾、聲調則無法出現顯著效應。同時亦於實驗中發現,中文聲調於規劃階段時,應是屬於詞彙結構上的聲調,而非純粹的聲韻調。zh_TW
dc.description.abstractSerial-ordering model and interactive processing model have long been discussed as whether people process languages in a sequential level of processing or the consequence of levels interacting altogether by looking at speech errors. Stroop technique (1935) in psychology could give linguistics some insights on the relation between lexical encoding and visual representation when people process colors. We tried to adapt this classical psychological experiment for an experiment of speech error elicitation. By means of controlling the phonological similarity, we created four experiments: color naming, color reading, Stroop naming, and homophonous naming tasks. The result first showed that even though phonological similarity did not induce significant difference in error amount and response time within single task, it still caused significant difference in these results among these tasks. \n Second, after analyzing the linguistic relation between targets and speech errors, we found that Stroop effect, syllable structure, phonotactic regularity, and tone induced apparent effects in error generation, while initial, rhyme, and vowel did not. Stroop effect not only provided the evidence from speech errors where character representation was an autonomous mechanism in lexical process, but also provided a fact that phonological effect would impacted differently on amount of error according to the type of visual task. It seems that interactive account could help explain the result easily, as the competition from dual visual inputs could be given a theoretical basis to account for the phonological dependency according to certain visual task which subjects were assigned to. \n Finally, we also discussed the issue of advanced planning unit in lexical process. In the shared unit task with Stroop technique, we found that only the units of tonal syllable and bare syllable could serve as possible planning units in lexical network, and tone in Chinese should be attributed to a type of lexical tone in planning, rather than a pure phonological tone. To sum up, the purpose of the study attempts to provide empirical evidence to examine the above issues.en_US
dc.description.tableofcontentsAcknowledges……………………………………………………iv\nVITA………………………………………………………………viii\nTable of Contents ……………………………………………x\nList of Tables ………………………………………………xii\nList of Figures …………………………………… ………xiv\nChinese Abstract ……………………………………………xv\nEnglish Abstract ……………………………………………xvi\nCHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION …………………………………1\nCHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW…………………………5\n2.1. Models of Speech Production ……………………5\n2.1.1. Utterance Generator Model of Speech Production …5\n2.1.2. Levelt’s Lexical Model ………………………9\n2.1.3. Interactive Activation Model ………………11\n2.1.4. Connectionist Model………………………………14\n2.2. Linguistic Effects in previous studies ……17\n2.2.1. Initialness Effect…………………………… …17\n2.2.2. Rhyme Effect ………………………………………18\n2.2.3. Phonological Similarity Effect…………… 19\n2.2.4. Syllable Structure Effect ……………………20\n2.2.5. Tone Effect …………………………………………21\n2.2.6. Phonotactic Regularity Effect……………… 22\n2.2.7. Stroop Effect ………………………………………23\n2.3. The Planning Unit in Sound Production………24\n2.4. Literature Summary and Research Hypothesis …25\nCHAPTER 3 METHODOLOGY…………………………………… 28\n3.1. General Methods of Task 1 ~ 4 …………………29\n3.1.1. Subjects ………………………………………………29\n3.1.2. Colors and Phonological Similarity ………31\n3.1.3. Test Items ……………………………………………44\n3.1.3.1. Task 1: Color Naming Test……………………45\n3.1.3.2. Task 2: Color Reading Test…………………47\n3.1.3.3. Task 3: Stroop Naming Test…………………48\n3.1.3.4. Task 4: Homophonous Naming Task …………50\n3.1.4. General Procedure and Data Analysis for Task 1~4 …53\n3.2. Task 5: Shared Units Naming Task ………………55\n3.2.1. Shared Unit: Onset ……………………………… 55\n3.2.2. Shared Unit: Vowel ………………………………58\n3.2.3. Shared Unit: Rhyme …………………………………60\n3.2.4. Shared Unit: Bare Syllable ……………………62\n3.2.5. Shared Unit: Bare Tone …………………………64\n3.2.6. Shared Unit: Tonal Syllable …………………67\n3.2.7. General Procedure of Shared Unit Naming Task …69\nCHAPTER 4 RESULTS AND DISCUSSIONS……………………71\n4.1. The Structure of Speech error and Reaction Time: Task 1~4 ……………………………………………………………………71\n4.2. Phonological Effect on Lexical Encoding ……83\n4.2.1 Within Task………………………………………………83\n4.2.2 Between Tasks……………………………………………84\n4.2.3 Generation of Speech Errors and RT in Stroop’s Tasks…………………………………………………………………87\n4.3. Linguistic Effects and Speech Errors: Test1~4 …88\n4.3.1. Initialness Effect …………………………………91\n4.3.2. Rhyme Effect ………………………………………… 92\n4.3.3. Tone Effect ……………………………………………93\n4.3.4. Syllable Structure Effect ………………………94\n4.3.5. Phonotactic Regularity Effect …………………96\n4.3.6. Stroop Effect …………………………………………96\n4.4. The Structure of Speech error and Reaction Time: Task 5 ………………………………………………………………………98\n4.5. Summary: Reflections on Lexical Process Models …105\nCHAPTER 5 CONCLUSION ………………………………………108\nReferences…………………………………………………………110\nAppendix 1: Stimuli Design…………………………………117\nAppendix 2: Overview of Results…………………………123zh_TW
dc.language.isoen_US-
dc.source.urihttp://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G0097555009en_US
dc.subject詞彙處理模型zh_TW
dc.subject史楚普效應zh_TW
dc.subject音韻近似度zh_TW
dc.subject語誤zh_TW
dc.subject語言規劃單位zh_TW
dc.subjectLexical modelen_US
dc.subjectStroop effecten_US
dc.subjectPhonological similarityen_US
dc.subjectSpeech errorsen_US
dc.subjectAdvanced planning uniten_US
dc.title中文顏色詞彙處理機制:心理語言學的實證zh_TW
dc.titleThe Lexical process of color terms in Chinese: Evidence from Psycholinguisticsen_US
dc.typethesisen
dc.relation.referenceBaddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 575–589.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceBerg, T. (2005). A Structural Account of Phonological Paraphasias. Brain and Language, 94, 104-129.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceBoomer, D. S. & Laver, J. D. M. (1968). Slips of the tongue. British Journal of Disorders of Communication 3, 2-12.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceBuckingham, H. W. (1980). On correlating aphasic errors with slips-of-tongue. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 199-220.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceButterworth, B. L. (1980). Evidence from pauses in speech. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production (Vol. 1, pp. 155-176). London: Academic Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceButterworth, B. L. (1982). Speech errors: Old date in search of new theories. In A. Culter (Ed.), Slips of tongue and language production. Amsterdam: Mouton.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceChen, J.-Y. (1999). The representation and processing of tone in Mandarin Chinese: Evidence from slips of the tongue. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 289-301.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceChen, J.-Y. & Dell, G. S. (2003). Word Form Encoding in Chinese Speech Production. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 45 (4), 313-322.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceDell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review 93 (3), 283-321.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceDell, G. S. (1988). The retrieval of phonological forms in production: Tests of predictions from a connectionist model. Journal of Memory and Language 27, 124-142.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceDell, G. S.& Gordon, J. K. (2003). Neighbors in the lexicon: Friends or foes? In N. O. Schiller and A. S. Meyer (Eds.), Phonetics and phonology in language comprehension and production: Differences and similarities. New York: Mouton de Grugter.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceDell, G. S.& Juliano, C. (1996). Computational models of phonological encoding. In T. Dijkstra & K. de Smedt (Eds.) Computational psycholinguistics: AI and connectionist models of human language processing (pp. 328-359). Taylor & Francis.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceDell, G. S.& Juliano & Govindjee, A. (1993). Structure and content in language production: A theory of frame constraints in phonological speech errors. Cognitive Science, 17, 149-195.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceDell, G. S. & O’Seaghdha, P. G. (1991). Mediated and convergent lexical priming in language production: A comment on Levelt at al. (1991). Psychological Review.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceDell, G. S. & O’Seaghdha, P. G. (1992). Stages of lexical access in language production. Cognition 42, 287-314.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceDell, G. S. & Reich, P. A. (1981) Stages in sentence production: An analysis of speech error data. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 611-629.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceDell, G. S., & Repka, R. J. (1992). Errors in inner speech. In B. J. Baars (Ed.), Experimental slips and human error: Exploring the architecture of volition, 237–262. New York: Plenum.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceDell, G. S. & Svec, W. R. (1997). Language Production and Serial Order: A Functional Analysis and a Model, Psychological Review, 104 (1), 123-147.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceDell, G. S. & Schwartz, M. F. & Martin, N. & Gagnon, D. A. (1997). Lexical Access in Aphasic and Nonaphasic Speakers. Psychological Review, 104 (4), 801-838.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceDell, G. S., Lawler, E. N., Harris, H. D., & Gordon, J. K. (2004). Models of errors of omission in aphasic naming. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21, 125–145.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceFay, D. & Cutler, A. (1977). The Phonology of Standard Chinese. Oxford University Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceForster, K. I. (1976). Accessing the mental lexicon. In F. J. Wales & E. Walker (Eds.) New Approaches to language mechanisms (pp.257-287). Amsterdam: North-Holland.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceFromkin, V. (1971). The nonanomalous nature of anomalous utterances. Linguistics 4, 47-68.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceFromkin (1973). Speech errors as linguistic evidence. The Hauge: Mouton.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceFromkin, V. & Ratner, N. B. (1998). Speech production. In J. B. Gleason & N. B. Ratner (Eds.), Psycholinguistics (pp. 309-346). California: Wadsworth.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceGarrett, M. F. (1980). Levels of processing in sentence production. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Speech production (Vol. 1). New York: Academic Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceGarrett, M. F. (1988). Processes in language production. In F. J. Newmeyer (Ed.), Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey III. Language: Psychological and biological aspects (pp.69-96). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceGordon, Jean K. (2002). Phonological neighborhood effects in aphasic speech errors: spontaneous and structured contexts. Brain and Language, 82, 113-145.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceHockett, C. F. (1967). Where the tongue slips, there slip I. In V. Fromkin (Ed.), Speech errors as linguistic evidence (pp.93-119). Netherlands: Mouton, the Hague.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceHotopf, W. H. N. (1980). Semantic similarity as a factor in whole-word slips of the tongue. In V. A. Fromkin (Ed.), Errors in linguistic performance: Slips of the tongue, ear, pen, and hand, 97–109. New York: Academic Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceHuang, C. T. James, Li, Y.-H. Audrey & Li, Y. (2008). The syntax of Chinese. Cambridge University Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceLevelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intension to articulation. Cambridge: MIT Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceLevelt, W. J. M. (ed.) (1993). Lexical Access in Speech Production. UK: Blackwell Publisher.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceLevelt, W. J. M. (1993). Accessing Words in Speech Production: Stages, Processes and Representations. Lexical Access in Speech Production, 1-22.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceLevelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–75.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceLevitt, A.G. & Healy, A.F. (1985). The roles of phonese frequency, similarity, and availability in the experimental elicitation of speech errors. Journal of Memory and Language 24, 717-733.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceLuce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: the Neighborhood Activation Model. Ear and Hearing, 1–36.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceMackay, D. G. (1970). Spoonerisms: The structure of errors in the serial order of speech. Neuropsychologia 8, 323-350.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceMackay, D. G. (1972). The structure of wrds and syllables: Evidence from errors in speech. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 210-227.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceMaddieson, Ian (1986). The size and structural of phonological inventories: analysis of UPSID. In Ohala, John & Jeri Jaeger (eds), Experimental Phonology. Academic Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceMartin, N., Gagnon, D. A., Schwartz, M. F., Dell, G. S. & Saffran, E. M. (1996). Phonological facilitation of semantic errors in normal and aphasic speakers. Language and Cognitive Processes 11 (3), 257-282.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceMartin, N., & Ayala, J. (2004). Measurements of auditoryverbal STM in aphasia: effects of task, item and word processing impairment. Brain and Language, 8, 464–483.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceMcClelland & Rumelhart (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review 88 (5), 375-407.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceMeringer, R. & Mayer, K. (1895). Versprechen und Verlesen: eine psychologisch-linguistische Studie. Stuttgart: Goschense Verlagsbuchhandlung.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceMeyer, A. S. (1990). The time course of phonological encoding in language production: The encoding of successive syllables of a word. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 524-545.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceMeyer, A. S. (1991). The time course of phonological encoding in language production: Phonological encoding inside a syllable. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 69-89.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceNooteboom, S.G. (1969). The tongue slips into patterns. Leyden Studies in Linguistics and Phonetics. The Hague: Mouton.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceOppenheim, G. M. & Dell, G. S. (2008). Inner speech slips exhibit lexical bias, but not the phonemic similarity effect. Cognition, 106, 528-537.zh_TW
dc.relation.referencePackard, J. L. (1986). Tone Production deficits in nonfluent aphasic Chinese speech, Brain and Language, 29, 212-223.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceRapp, D. N. & Samuel, A.G. (2002). A reason to rhyme: Phonological and semantic influences on lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 28 (3), 564-571.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceRoelofs, A. (1996). Computationa; models of lemma retrieval. In T. Dijkstra & K. de Smedt (Eds.) Computational Psycholinguistics: AI and connectionist mode4ls of human language processing (pp.308-327). Taylor & Francis.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceRoelofs, A. (1997) The WEAVER model of word-form encoding in speech production.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceRoelofs, A., & Meyer, A.S. (1998). Metrical structure in planning the production of spoken words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 922–939.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceSchwartz, M. F. & Dell, G. S. & Martin, N. (2004). Testing the interactive two-step model of lexical access: Part I. picture naming, Brain and Language, 91, 71-72.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceSevald, C. A., Dell, G. S., &Cole, J. (1995). Syllable structure in speech production: Are syllable chunks or schemas? Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 807-820.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceShattuck-Hufnagel (1979). Speech errors as evidence for a serial ordering mechanism in sentence production. In W. E. Cooper & E. C. T. Walkers (Eds.), Sentence processing (pp.295-342). Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Assiciates.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceShattuck-Hufnagel (1986). The representation of phonologoical information during speech production planning: Evidence from vowel errors in spontaneous speech. Phonology Yearbook 3, 117-149.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceShattuck-Hufnagel (1987). The role of word onset consonants in speech production planning: New evidence from speech error patterns. In E. Keller & M. Gopnik (Eds.), Motor and sensory processing in language (pp. 17-51). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceSchwartz, M. F. (1994). Disordered Speech Production in Aphasic and Normal Speakers. Brain and Language, 47, 52-88.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceSokolov, A. N. (1972). Inner speech and thought. New York: Plenum.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceStemberger, J. P. (1983). Speech errors and theoretical phonology: A review. Bloomington: Indiana Linguistics Club.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceStemberger, J. P. (1984). Structural Errors in Normal and Agrammatic Speech. Cognitve Neuropsychology, 1 (4), 281-313.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceStemberger, J. P. (1985). An interactive activation model of language production. In W. E. Andrew (Ed.), Progress in the psychology of language (pp. 143-186). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd., Publishers.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceStemberger, J. P. (1989). Speech errors in early child language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 28( 2), 164-188.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceStroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643-662.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceVitevitch, M. S. (1997). The neighborhood characteristics of malapropisms. Language and Speech, 40, 211–228.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceVitevitch, M. S., & Luce, P. A. (1998). When words compete: levels of processing in perception of spoken words. Psychological Science, 9, 325–329.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceVitevitch, M. S. (2002, July). The influence of phonological similarity neighborhoods on speech production. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn, 28(4), 735–747.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceVitevitch, M. S., Armbruster, J., & Chu, S. (2004). Sublexical and lexical representations in speech production: effects of phonotactic probability and onset density. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn, 30(2), 514–529.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceVitevitch, M. S., & Sommers, M. S. (2003). The facilitative influence of phonological similarity and neighborhood frequency in speech production in younger and older adults. Memory and Cognition, 31, 491–504.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceVitevitch, M. S., & Stamer, M. K. (2006). The curious case of competition in Spanish speech production. Language & Cognitive Processes, 21(6), 760–770.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceWan, I. P. & Jeri J. (1998) Speexh errors and the representation of tone in Mandarin Chinese. Phonology, 15, 417-461.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceWan, I. P. (1999). Mandarin phonology: Evidence from speech errors. Ph D. Dissertation.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceWan, I. P. (2007). Aphonological Investigation in Speech Errors and Aphasic Speech in Mandarin. Taipei: Crane.zh_TW
dc.relation.referenceWells, R. (1951). Predicting slips of the tongue. Yale Scientific Magazine, 3, 9-30.zh_TW
item.openairecristypehttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_46ec-
item.languageiso639-1en_US-
item.grantfulltextopen-
item.openairetypethesis-
item.cerifentitytypePublications-
item.fulltextWith Fulltext-
Appears in Collections:學位論文
Files in This Item:
File SizeFormat
500901.pdf1.59 MBAdobe PDF2View/Open
Show simple item record

Google ScholarTM

Check


Items in DSpace are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.