學術產出-學位論文
文章檢視/開啟
書目匯出
-
題名 慢性疼痛病人的敵意覺知、寂寞感與疼痛程度之關聯探究
Exploring the Relationship among Hostility, Loneliness, and Pain Level in Chronic Pain Patients作者 林彤 貢獻者 吳治勳
林彤關鍵詞 慢性疼痛
敵意
寂寞感
chronic pain
hostility
loneliness日期 2022 上傳時間 1-八月-2022 18:10:48 (UTC+8) 摘要 研究背景:慢性疼痛影響個人生活的生理、心理與社會層面,衍生負面情緒,降低個人的生活品質。慢性疼痛病人面對環境中具威脅性的訊號時,形成一系列與敵意有關之認知、情緒與行為歷程,並可能因此延續與強化病人的疼痛程度。本研究藉由文獻回顧,推測敵意與疼痛程度之間應存在顯著關聯,並將寂寞感作為一主觀社會變項,探討寂寞感如何影響敵意對於疼痛程度的關係。為此,本研究將進行以橫斷性與縱貫性資料檢驗變項間的關聯。研究方法:本研究參與者來自北部某醫學中心疼痛門診之慢性疼痛病人。參與者將進行多時間點的評估,每次皆填答基本資料與疼痛評估,台灣疼痛評估量表、慢性病負向情緒篩檢量表、UCLA-3寂寞量表、及短式華人敵意量表。研究一將採用第一次評估之樣本資料進行橫斷式分析。探討敵意與疼痛之同時性關係時,先以皮爾森相關檢驗整體敵意與整體疼痛程度之相關性,再以路徑分析探討敵意四因素「敵意認知」、「敵意情感」、「表達敵意」、及「壓抑敵意」,與疼痛四因素「疼痛衝擊」、「疼痛-無助」、「疼痛-丟臉」、「疼痛-逃避」之關係;探討寂寞感、敵意與疼痛程度之關聯時,先以線性迴歸檢驗寂寞感是否能調節整體敵意與整體疼痛程度之關係,再根據寂寞感分數將參與者分為高/低寂寞感組,以多組別分析檢驗寂寞感對敵意四因素與疼痛四因素之關係是否具調節效果。研究二則採用第一次(T0)與三個月後第二次評估(T1)的追蹤樣本資料進行縱貫式分析,先以路徑分析檢驗敵意四因素(T0)與疼痛四因素(T1)的時序性關係,並以多組別分析檢驗寂寞感對於敵意四因素(T0)與疼痛四因素(T1)時序性關係的調節效果,最後再將寂寞感(T1)作為中介變項,檢驗寂寞感對於敵意四因素(T0)與疼痛四因素(T1)時序性關係的中介效果。研究結果:研究一共納入280位慢性疼痛病人。在敵意與疼痛的同時性關係中,整體敵意與整體疼痛程度顯著相關,路徑分析結果顯示「敵意情感」可正向預測「疼痛-逃避」,「壓抑敵意」可正向預測「疼痛衝擊」、「疼痛-無助」、「疼痛-丟臉」。以寂寞感作為調節變項,結果顯示寂寞感對於整體敵意與整體疼痛程度不具顯著調節效果。在多組別路徑分析結果中,寂寞感對敵意四因素與疼痛四因素之關係雖有部分調節效果,但效果於統計上皆未達顯著。研究二共納入167位病人之追蹤樣本。結果發現在敵意與疼痛在時序性關係中,「敵意認知T0」可正向預測「疼痛-丟臉T1」,「敵意情感T0」可正向預測「疼痛衝擊T1」,「壓抑敵意T0」可正向預測「疼痛衝擊T1」與「疼痛-無助T1」。以寂寞感T1作為調節變項時,寂寞感對敵意四因素T0與疼痛四因素T1之時序性關係雖有部分調節效果,但於統計上皆未達顯著。以寂寞感T1作為中介變項時,寂寞感可完全中介「敵意認知T0」、「壓抑敵意T0」兩個敵意面向對後續「疼痛衝擊T1」、「疼痛-丟臉T1」的關係。討論:本研究發現敵意對疼痛具有部分的預測效果,且相較同時性的關係,敵意與疼痛在時序性關係有更多面向之預測效果。另外,本研究發現寂寞感雖然無法作為調節變項影響敵意與疼痛的關係,但可作為中介變項部分影響敵意與疼痛的關係。綜合上述,當慢性疼痛病人抱持高敵意之認知思考,或傾向運用高強度之敵意壓抑行為,上述敵意表現將增強病人後續的寂寞感,強化後續疼痛讓病人感到丟臉、沒有面子等感受,並且放大慢性疼痛造成的身心衝擊。本研究藉由寂寞感、敵意與疼痛關係之多面向探討,增進對台灣慢性疼痛病人心理社會狀態之理解,並盼能藉此對病人的疼痛與生活適應有所助益。
Background: Chronic pain has strong influence on the physical, psychological and social aspects of patients’ life, which giving rise to negative emotions and reducing quality of life. When faced with threat signals in the environment, chronic pain patients initiate a series of cognitive, emotional and behavioral process related to hostility, which may prolong and intensify their pain level. Based of literature review, this study presumes that there is a significant relationship between hostility and pain level. This study also uses loneliness as a subjective social variable to explore how loneliness affects the relationship between hostility and pain level.Methods: Participants in this study were chronic pain patients in the pain clinic of a medical center in north Taiwan. Patients were asked to complete surveys at baseline(T0) and 3-month follow-up(T1.) They filled out the basic pain assessment, "the Pain Assessment Scale Taiwan," "the Negative Emotions due to Chronic Illness Screening Test," "the UCLA-3 Loneliness Scale-3", and "the Chinese Hostility Inventory-Short Form" in each survey. In study 1, T0 data were used for cross-sectional analysis. To investigate the simultaneous relation between hostility and pain, Pearson`s correlation was used to test the correlation between overall hostility and overall pain level. Path analysis was used to test the relationship among four factors of hostility ("hostile cognition," "hostile affection," "expressive hostility behavior," and "suppressive hostility behavior") and four factors of pain ("pain impact," "pain-helplessness," "pain-losing-face," and "pain-avoidance.")To explore the simultaneous relationship among hostility, pain, and loneliness, first linear regression was used to test whether loneliness could moderate the relationship between overall hostility and overall pain level. Then, after dividing into high/low loneliness groups according to their loneliness score, multi-group path analysis was used to test whether loneliness could moderate the relationship among the factors of hostility and pain. In study 2, T0 and T1 data were used for longitudinal analysis. First, to investigate their temporal association, path analysis was used to test the relationship among the factors of hostility(T0) and pain(T1.) Then multi-group path analysis was used to test whether loneliness(T1) could be the moderator among the factors of hostility(T0) and pain(T1.) Finally, path analysis was used to test whether loneliness(T1) could be the mediator among the factors of hostility(T0) and pain(T1.)Results: A total of 280 chronic pain patients were included in study 1. Regarding the simultaneous relationship between hostility and pain, overall hostility is significantly correlated with overall pain level. Path analysis results show that "hostile affections" can positively predict "pain-avoidance" "suppressive hostility behavior" positively predicts "pain impact," "pain-helplessness," and "pain-losing-face." When used loneliness as a moderator, the results showed that loneliness had no significant moderating effect between overall hostility and overall pain level. Through the multi-group path analysis, loneliness has partial but not significant moderating effects on the relationship among the four factors of hostility and the four factors of pain. A total of 167 follow-up samples were included in study 2. Regarding the temporal relationship between hostility and pain, path analysis results show that "hostile cognition(T0)" can positively predict "pain-losing-face(T1)" "hostile affection(T0)" can positively predict "pain impact(T1)", "pain-helplessness(T1)" "suppressive hostility behavior(T0)" positively predict "pain impact(T1)", "pain-helplessness(T1)." When used loneliness(T1) as a moderator, loneliness has partial but not significant moderating effects on the temporal relationship among the four factors of hostility(T0) and the four factors of pain(T1) through the multi-group path analysis. When used loneliness(T1) as a mediator, loneliness can fully mediate the temporal relationship between "hostile cognition(T0)"/"suppressive hostility behavior(T0)" and the "pain impact(T1)"/"pain-losing-face(T1)."Discussion: According to cross-sectional and longitudinal analysis, this study finds that hostility factors can be more predictive and influence more aspects of pain in longitudinal analysis. Besides, loneliness substantially affects the relationship between hostility and pain when regarded as a mediator. In conclusion, when chronic pain patients hold higher hostility cognitive thinking in mind or tend to use suppressive hostility behaviors frequently, these hostility presentations would increase loneliness subsequently, making the patient feel shame on themselves, amplifying the physical and psychological impact of pain on patients. By exploring the relationship among hostility, pain, and loneliness in different scopes, this study expands the understanding of the psychosocial context of chronic pain patients in Taiwan, looking forward to helping patients with chronic pain coping and general life adaptation.參考文獻 吳治勳,林至芃,楊啟正,葉芷圻。(2017)。臨床情境下之全面性疼痛評估:「台灣疼痛評估量表」之發展與心理計量特性建立。取自https://www.grb.gov.tw/search/planDetail?id=11908191林宜美、翁嘉英。(2002)。冠狀動脈心臟病與頭痛的敵意型態比較。中華心理學刊,44(2),211–226。翁嘉英、林宜美、呂碧鴻、陳秀蓉、吳英璋、鄭逸如。(2008)。「短式華人敵意量表」之發展與信效度考驗。測驗學刊,55(3),463–487。 https://doi.org/10.7108/PT.200812.0002陳梓、喻惠丹。(2018)。慢性病負性情緒篩查量表在冠心病患者中的信效度檢驗。中國臨床心理學雜誌,26(4),666–670。Anema, J. R., Schellart, A. J., Cassidy, J. D., Loisel, P., Veerman, T. J., & Van der Beek, A. J. (2009). Can cross country differences in return-to-work after chronic occupational back pain be explained? An exploratory analysis on disability policies in a six country cohort study. Journal of occupational rehabilitation, 19(4), 419-426. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10926-009-9202-3Asmundson, J. G., &Katz, J. (2009). Understanding the co-occurrence of anxiety disorders and chronic pain: state-of-the-art. Depression and Anxiety, 26(10), 888–901. https://doi.org/10.1002/DA.20600Averill, J. R. (1983). Studies on anger and aggression: Implications for theories of emotion. American Psychologist, 38(11), 1145–1160. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.38.11.1145Bair, M. J., Robinson, R. L., Katon, W., &Kroenke, K. (2003). Depression and Pain Comorbidity: A Literature Review. Archives of Internal Medicine, 163(20), 2433–2445. https://doi.org/10.1001/ARCHINTE.163.20.2433Barefoot, J. C. (1992). Developments in the measurement of hostility. In Hostility, coping, & health. (pp. 13–31). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10105-001Berkowitz, L., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2004). Toward an understanding of the determinants of anger. Emotion, 4(2), 107. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.4.2.107Boggero, I. A., Sturgeon, J. A., Arewasikporn, A., Castro, S. A., King, C. D., &Segerstrom, S. C. (2019). Associations of Pain Intensity and Frequency With Loneliness, Hostility, and Social Functioning: Cross-Sectional, Longitudinal, and Within-Person Relationships. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 26(2), 217–229. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-019-09776-5Bruehl, S., Chung, O. Y., Ward, P., Johnson, B., &McCubbin, J. A. (2002). The relationship between resting blood pressure and acute pain sensitivity in healthy normotensives and chronic back pain sufferers: The effects of opioid blockade. Pain, 100(1–2), 191–201. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00295-6Bruehl, S., Liu, X., Burns, J. W., Chont, M., &Jamison, R. N. (2012). Associations between daily chronic pain intensity, daily anger expression, and trait anger expressiveness: An ecological momentary assessment study. Pain, 153(12), 2352–2358. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2012.08.001Burke, A. L., Mathias, J. L., & Denson, L. A. (2015). Psychological functioning of people living with chronic pain: A meta‐analytic review. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 54(3), 345-360. https://doi.org/10.1111/BJC.12078Burns, J. W., Bruehl, S., &Chont, M. (2014). Anger regulation style, anger arousal and acute pain sensitivity: Evidence for an endogenous opioid “triggering” model. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 37(4), 642–653. https://doi.org/10.1007/s-10865-0139511-zBurns, J. W., Bruehl, S., &Quartana, P. J. (2006). Anger management style and hostility among patients with chronic pain: Effects on symptom-specific physiological reactivity during anger- and sadness- recall interviews. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68(5), 786–793. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.psy.0000238211.89198.e4Burns, J. W., Gerhart, J. I., Bruehl, S., Peterson, K. M., Smith, D. A., Porter, L. S., ... & Keefe, F. J. (2015). Anger arousal and behavioral anger regulation in everyday life among patients with chronic low back pain: Relationships to patient pain and function. Health Psychology, 34(5),547. https://doi.org/10.1037/HEA0000091Burns, J. W., Gerhart, J. I., Bruehl, S., Post, K. M., Smith, D. A., Porter, L. S., ... & Keefe, F. J. (2016). Anger arousal and behavioral anger regulation in everyday life among people with chronic low back pain: Relationships with spouse responses and negative affect. Health Psychology, 35(1),29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/hea0000221Burns, J. W., Johnson, B. J., Devine, J., Mahoney, N., &Pawl, R. (1998). Anger management style and the prediction of treatment outcome among male and female chronic pain patients. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(11), 1051–1062. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00080-1Buss, A. H., &Durkee, A. (1957). An inventory for assessing different kinds of hostility. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 21(4), 343.Buss, A. H., &Perry, M. (1992). The Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(3), 452–459. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.452Cacioppo, J. T., &Hawkley, L. C. (2009). Perceived social isolation and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(10), 447–454. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.005Cacioppo, S., Grippo, A. J., London, S., Goossens, L., &Cacioppo, J. T. (2015). Loneliness: Clinical Import and Interventions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 238–249. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615570616Eckhardt, C., Norlander, B., &Deffenbacher, J. (2004). The assessment of anger and hostility: a critical review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 9(1), 17–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1359-1789(02)00116-7Edwards, R. R., Dworkin, R. H., Sullivan, M. D., Turk, D. C., &Wasan, A. D. (2016). The Role of Psychosocial Processes in the Development and Maintenance of Chronic Pain. Journal of Pain, 17(9), T70–T92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2016.01.001Eisenberger, N. I. (2012). The pain of social disconnection: Examining the shared neural underpinnings of physical and social pain. In Nature Reviews Neuroscience (Vol. 13, Issue 6, pp. 421–434). Nature Publishing Group. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3231Eisenberger, N. I., &Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why rejection hurts: A common neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(7), 294–300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.010Engel, G. L. (1977). The need for a new medical model: A challenge for biomedicine. Science, 196(4286), 129–136. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.847460Fernandez, E., &Turk, D. C. (1995). The scope and significance of anger in the experience of chronic pain. In Pain (Vol. 61, Issue 2, pp. 165–175). https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(95)00192-UFillingim, R. B., King, C. D., Ribeiro-Dasilva, M. C., Rahim-Williams, B., &Riley, J. L. (2009). Sex, Gender, and Pain: A Review of Recent Clinical and Experimental Findings. In Journal of Pain (Vol. 10, Issue 5, pp. 447–485). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2008.12.001Gatchel, R. J. (2004). Comorbidity of chronic pain and mental health disorders: the biopsychosocial perspective. American Psychologist, 59(8), 795. https://doi.org/10.1037/00303-066X.59.8.795Gerrits, M. M. J. G., vanMarwijk, H. W. J., vanOppen, P., van derHorst, H., &Penninx, B. W. J. H. (2015). Longitudinal association between pain, and depression and anxiety over four years. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 78(1), 64–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JPSYCHORES.2014.10.011Hemingway, H., &Marmot, M. (1999). Evidence based cardiology: Psychosocial factors in the aetiology and prognosis of coronary heart disease: systematic review of prospective cohort studies. BMJ : British Medical Journal, 318(7196), 1460. https://doi.org/10.1136/BMJ.318.7196.1460Huang, Y. H., Wu, C. H., Chen, H. J., Cheng, Y. R., Hung, F. C., Leung, K. K., Lue, B. H., Chen, C. Y., Chiu, T. Y., &Wu, Y. C. (2017). Quick screening tool for patients with severe negative emotional reactions to chronic illness: Psychometric study of the negative emotions due to chronic illness screening test (NECIS). Family Practice, 35(1), 34–40. https://doi.org/10.1093/fampra/cmx076Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). A short scale for measuring loneliness in large surveys: Results from two population-based studies. Research on aging, 26(6), 655-672. https://doi.org/10.1177/0164027504268574IASP. (2020). IASP Announces Revised Definition of Pain. https://www.iasp-pain.org/publications/iasp-news/iasp-announces-revised-definition-of-pain/?ItemNumber=10475Jamison, R. N., &Virts, K. L. (1990). The influence of family support on chronic pain. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28(4), 283–287. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(90)90079-XJensen, M. P., Moore, M. R., Bockow, T. B., Ehde, D. M., & Engel, J. M. (2011). Psychosocial factors and adjustment to chronic pain in persons with physical disabilities: a systematic review. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation, 92(1), 146-160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2010.09.021Leeuw, M., Goossens, M. E. J. B., Linton, S. J., Crombez, G., Boersma, K., &Vlaeyen, J. W. S. (2007). The fear-avoidance model of musculoskeletal pain: current state of scientific evidence. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 30(1), 77–94. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-006-9085-0Leonard, M. T., Cano, A., &Johansen, A. B. (2006). Chronic Pain in a Couples Context: A Review and Integration of Theoretical Models and Empirical Evidence. The Journal of Pain, 7(6), 377–390. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JPAIN.2006.01.442López-Martínez, A. E., Esteve-Zarazaga, R., &Ramírez-Maestre, C. (2008). Perceived Social Support and Coping Responses Are Independent Variables Explaining Pain Adjustment Among Chronic Pain Patients. Journal of Pain, 9(4), 373–379. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2007.12.002McWilliams, L. A., Cox, B. J., &Enns, M. W. (2003). Mood and anxiety disorders associated with chronic pain: An examination in a nationally representative sample. Pain, 106(1–2), 127–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00301-4Meints, S. M., &Edwards, R. R. (2018). Evaluating psychosocial contributions to chronic pain outcomes. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 87, 168–182. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PNPBP.2018.01.017Melzack, R., Wall, P. D., &others. (1965). Pain Mechanisms: A New Theory: A gate control system modulates sensory input from the skin before it evokes pain perception and response. Science, 150(3699), 971–979. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.150.3699.971Meng, J., Wang, X., Wei, D., &Qiu, J. (2020). State loneliness is associated with emotional hypervigilance in daily life: A network analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 165, 110154. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PAID.2020.110154Mills, S. E. E., Nicolson, K. P., &Smith, B. H. (2019). Chronic pain: a review of its epidemiology and associated factors in population-based studies. British Journal of Anaesthesia, 123(2), e273–e283. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BJA.2019.03.023Moayedi, M., &Davis, K. D. (2013). Theories of pain: From specificity to gate control. Journal of Neurophysiology, 109(1), 5–12. https://doi.org/10.1152/JN.00457.2012Ohayon, M. M., &Schatzberg, A. F. (2010). Chronic pain and major depressive disorder in the general population. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 44(7), 454–461. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JPSYCHIRES.2009.10.013Okifuji, A., Turk, D. C., &Curran, S. L. (1999). Anger in chronic pain: Investigations of anger targets and intensity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 47(1), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(99)00006-9Peplau, L. A., Perlman, D. (1982). Perspectives on loneliness. In Peplau, L. A., Perlman, D. , Loneliness: A sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy , (pp. 1–8.)Qualter, P., Vanhalst, J., Harris, R., VanRoekel, E., Lodder, G., Bangee, M., Maes, M., &Verhagen, M. (2015). Loneliness Across the Life Span. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 250–264. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615568999Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., &Cutrona, C. E. (1980). The revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(3), 472–480. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.39.3.472Rustøen, T., Wahl, A. K., Hanestad, B. R., Lerdal, A., Paul, S., &Miaskowski, C. (2005). Age and the experience of chronic pain: Differences in health and quality of life among younger, middle-aged, and older adults. Clinical Journal of Pain, 21(6), 513–523. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.ajp.0000146217.31780.efSegel-Karpas, D., &Ayalon, L. (2020). Loneliness and hostility in older adults: A cross-lagged model. Psychology and Aging, 35(2), 169. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000417Smith, T. W. (1992). Hostility and health: Current status of a psychosomatic hypothesis. Health Psychology, 11(3), 139–150. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.11.3.139Smith, T. W., Glazer, K., Ruiz, J. M., &Gallo, L. C. (2004). Hostility, Anger, Aggressiveness, and Coronary Heart Disease: An Interpersonal Perspective on Personality, Emotion, and Health. Journal of Personality, 72(6), 1217–1270. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1467-6494.2004.00296.XSmith, T. W., &Weihs, K. (2019). Emotion, Social Relationships, and Physical Health: Concepts, Methods, and Evidence for an Integrative Perspective. Psychosomatic Medicine, 81(8), 681–693. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000739Spielberger, C. D., Jacobs, G., Russell, S., &Crane, R. S. (1983). Assessment of anger: The state-trait anger scale. Advances in Personality Assessment, 2, 161–189.Spielberger, C. D., Johnson, E. H., Jacobs, G. A., Krasner, S. S., Oesterle, S. E., &Worden, T. J. (1986). The anger expression (AX) scale. Tampa, FL: Center for Research in Behavioral Medicine and Community Psychology.Spielberger, C. D., Reheiser, E. C., &Sydeman, S. J. (1995). Measuring the experience, expression, and control of anger. Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric Nursing, 18(3), 207–232. https://doi.org/10.3109/01460869509087271Spithoven, A. W. M., Bijttebier, P., &Goossens, L. (2017). It is all in their mind: A review on information processing bias in lonely individuals. Clinical Psychology Review, 58, 97–114. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CPR.2017.10.003Steingrímsdóttir, Ó. A., Landmark, T., Macfarlane, G. J., &Nielsen, C. S. (2017). Defining chronic pain in epidemiological studies: A systematic review and meta-analysis. In Pain (Vol. 158, Issue 11, pp. 2092–2107). Lippincott Williams and Wilkins. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001009Sturgeon, J. A., &Zautra, A. J. (2016). Social pain and physical pain: shared paths to resilience. In Pain management (Vol. 6, Issue 1, pp. 63–74). Future Science Group. https://doi.org/10.2217/pmt.15.56Treede, R. D., Rief, W., Barke, A., Aziz, Q., Bennett, M. I., Benoliel, R., Cohen, M., Evers, S., Finnerup, N. B., First, M. B., Giamberardino, M. A., Kaasa, S., Korwisi, B., Kosek, E., Lavand’Homme, P., Nicholas, M., Perrot, S., Scholz, J., Schug, S., …Wang, S. J. (2019). Chronic pain as a symptom or a disease: The IASP Classification of Chronic Pain for the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11). Pain, 160(1), 19–27. https://doi.org/10.1097/J.PAIN.0000000000001384Victor, C. R., Scambler, S. J., Bowling, A., &Bond, J. (2005). The prevalence of, and risk factors for, loneliness in later life: a survey of older people in Great Britain. Ageing & Society, 25(6), 357–375. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X04003332Vlaeyen, J. W. S., Kole-Snijders, A. M. J., Boeren, R. G. B., &vanEek, H. (1995). Fear of movement/(re)injury in chronic low back pain and its relation to behavioral performance. Pain, 62(3), 363–372. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(94)00279-NVlaeyen, J. W. S., Kole-Snijders, A. M. J., Rotteveel, A. M., Ruesink, R., &Heuts, P. H. T. G. (1995). The role of fear of movement/(re)injury in pain disability. Journal of Occupational Rehabilitation, 5(4), 235–252. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02109988Vlaeyen, J. W. S., &Linton, S. J. (2000). Fear-avoidance and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: A state of the art. In Pain (Vol. 85, Issue 3, pp. 317–332). No longer published by Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00242-0Weiss, R. S. (1973). Loneliness: The experience of emotional and social isolation. The MIT Press.WHO. (2021). ICD-11 Reference Guide, International Classification of Diseases for Mortality and Morbidity Statistics. http://id.who.int/icd/entity/437815624Wiech, K., & Tracey, I. (2009). The influence of negative emotions on pain: behavioral effects and neural mechanisms. Neuroimage, 47(3), 987-994. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.059 描述 碩士
國立政治大學
心理學系
107752003資料來源 http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G0107752003 資料類型 thesis dc.contributor.advisor 吳治勳 zh_TW dc.contributor.author (作者) 林彤 zh_TW dc.creator (作者) 林彤 zh_TW dc.date (日期) 2022 en_US dc.date.accessioned 1-八月-2022 18:10:48 (UTC+8) - dc.date.available 1-八月-2022 18:10:48 (UTC+8) - dc.date.issued (上傳時間) 1-八月-2022 18:10:48 (UTC+8) - dc.identifier (其他 識別碼) G0107752003 en_US dc.identifier.uri (URI) http://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/141171 - dc.description (描述) 碩士 zh_TW dc.description (描述) 國立政治大學 zh_TW dc.description (描述) 心理學系 zh_TW dc.description (描述) 107752003 zh_TW dc.description.abstract (摘要) 研究背景:慢性疼痛影響個人生活的生理、心理與社會層面,衍生負面情緒,降低個人的生活品質。慢性疼痛病人面對環境中具威脅性的訊號時,形成一系列與敵意有關之認知、情緒與行為歷程,並可能因此延續與強化病人的疼痛程度。本研究藉由文獻回顧,推測敵意與疼痛程度之間應存在顯著關聯,並將寂寞感作為一主觀社會變項,探討寂寞感如何影響敵意對於疼痛程度的關係。為此,本研究將進行以橫斷性與縱貫性資料檢驗變項間的關聯。研究方法:本研究參與者來自北部某醫學中心疼痛門診之慢性疼痛病人。參與者將進行多時間點的評估,每次皆填答基本資料與疼痛評估,台灣疼痛評估量表、慢性病負向情緒篩檢量表、UCLA-3寂寞量表、及短式華人敵意量表。研究一將採用第一次評估之樣本資料進行橫斷式分析。探討敵意與疼痛之同時性關係時,先以皮爾森相關檢驗整體敵意與整體疼痛程度之相關性,再以路徑分析探討敵意四因素「敵意認知」、「敵意情感」、「表達敵意」、及「壓抑敵意」,與疼痛四因素「疼痛衝擊」、「疼痛-無助」、「疼痛-丟臉」、「疼痛-逃避」之關係;探討寂寞感、敵意與疼痛程度之關聯時,先以線性迴歸檢驗寂寞感是否能調節整體敵意與整體疼痛程度之關係,再根據寂寞感分數將參與者分為高/低寂寞感組,以多組別分析檢驗寂寞感對敵意四因素與疼痛四因素之關係是否具調節效果。研究二則採用第一次(T0)與三個月後第二次評估(T1)的追蹤樣本資料進行縱貫式分析,先以路徑分析檢驗敵意四因素(T0)與疼痛四因素(T1)的時序性關係,並以多組別分析檢驗寂寞感對於敵意四因素(T0)與疼痛四因素(T1)時序性關係的調節效果,最後再將寂寞感(T1)作為中介變項,檢驗寂寞感對於敵意四因素(T0)與疼痛四因素(T1)時序性關係的中介效果。研究結果:研究一共納入280位慢性疼痛病人。在敵意與疼痛的同時性關係中,整體敵意與整體疼痛程度顯著相關,路徑分析結果顯示「敵意情感」可正向預測「疼痛-逃避」,「壓抑敵意」可正向預測「疼痛衝擊」、「疼痛-無助」、「疼痛-丟臉」。以寂寞感作為調節變項,結果顯示寂寞感對於整體敵意與整體疼痛程度不具顯著調節效果。在多組別路徑分析結果中,寂寞感對敵意四因素與疼痛四因素之關係雖有部分調節效果,但效果於統計上皆未達顯著。研究二共納入167位病人之追蹤樣本。結果發現在敵意與疼痛在時序性關係中,「敵意認知T0」可正向預測「疼痛-丟臉T1」,「敵意情感T0」可正向預測「疼痛衝擊T1」,「壓抑敵意T0」可正向預測「疼痛衝擊T1」與「疼痛-無助T1」。以寂寞感T1作為調節變項時,寂寞感對敵意四因素T0與疼痛四因素T1之時序性關係雖有部分調節效果,但於統計上皆未達顯著。以寂寞感T1作為中介變項時,寂寞感可完全中介「敵意認知T0」、「壓抑敵意T0」兩個敵意面向對後續「疼痛衝擊T1」、「疼痛-丟臉T1」的關係。討論:本研究發現敵意對疼痛具有部分的預測效果,且相較同時性的關係,敵意與疼痛在時序性關係有更多面向之預測效果。另外,本研究發現寂寞感雖然無法作為調節變項影響敵意與疼痛的關係,但可作為中介變項部分影響敵意與疼痛的關係。綜合上述,當慢性疼痛病人抱持高敵意之認知思考,或傾向運用高強度之敵意壓抑行為,上述敵意表現將增強病人後續的寂寞感,強化後續疼痛讓病人感到丟臉、沒有面子等感受,並且放大慢性疼痛造成的身心衝擊。本研究藉由寂寞感、敵意與疼痛關係之多面向探討,增進對台灣慢性疼痛病人心理社會狀態之理解,並盼能藉此對病人的疼痛與生活適應有所助益。 zh_TW dc.description.abstract (摘要) Background: Chronic pain has strong influence on the physical, psychological and social aspects of patients’ life, which giving rise to negative emotions and reducing quality of life. When faced with threat signals in the environment, chronic pain patients initiate a series of cognitive, emotional and behavioral process related to hostility, which may prolong and intensify their pain level. Based of literature review, this study presumes that there is a significant relationship between hostility and pain level. This study also uses loneliness as a subjective social variable to explore how loneliness affects the relationship between hostility and pain level.Methods: Participants in this study were chronic pain patients in the pain clinic of a medical center in north Taiwan. Patients were asked to complete surveys at baseline(T0) and 3-month follow-up(T1.) They filled out the basic pain assessment, "the Pain Assessment Scale Taiwan," "the Negative Emotions due to Chronic Illness Screening Test," "the UCLA-3 Loneliness Scale-3", and "the Chinese Hostility Inventory-Short Form" in each survey. In study 1, T0 data were used for cross-sectional analysis. To investigate the simultaneous relation between hostility and pain, Pearson`s correlation was used to test the correlation between overall hostility and overall pain level. Path analysis was used to test the relationship among four factors of hostility ("hostile cognition," "hostile affection," "expressive hostility behavior," and "suppressive hostility behavior") and four factors of pain ("pain impact," "pain-helplessness," "pain-losing-face," and "pain-avoidance.")To explore the simultaneous relationship among hostility, pain, and loneliness, first linear regression was used to test whether loneliness could moderate the relationship between overall hostility and overall pain level. Then, after dividing into high/low loneliness groups according to their loneliness score, multi-group path analysis was used to test whether loneliness could moderate the relationship among the factors of hostility and pain. In study 2, T0 and T1 data were used for longitudinal analysis. First, to investigate their temporal association, path analysis was used to test the relationship among the factors of hostility(T0) and pain(T1.) Then multi-group path analysis was used to test whether loneliness(T1) could be the moderator among the factors of hostility(T0) and pain(T1.) Finally, path analysis was used to test whether loneliness(T1) could be the mediator among the factors of hostility(T0) and pain(T1.)Results: A total of 280 chronic pain patients were included in study 1. Regarding the simultaneous relationship between hostility and pain, overall hostility is significantly correlated with overall pain level. Path analysis results show that "hostile affections" can positively predict "pain-avoidance" "suppressive hostility behavior" positively predicts "pain impact," "pain-helplessness," and "pain-losing-face." When used loneliness as a moderator, the results showed that loneliness had no significant moderating effect between overall hostility and overall pain level. Through the multi-group path analysis, loneliness has partial but not significant moderating effects on the relationship among the four factors of hostility and the four factors of pain. A total of 167 follow-up samples were included in study 2. Regarding the temporal relationship between hostility and pain, path analysis results show that "hostile cognition(T0)" can positively predict "pain-losing-face(T1)" "hostile affection(T0)" can positively predict "pain impact(T1)", "pain-helplessness(T1)" "suppressive hostility behavior(T0)" positively predict "pain impact(T1)", "pain-helplessness(T1)." When used loneliness(T1) as a moderator, loneliness has partial but not significant moderating effects on the temporal relationship among the four factors of hostility(T0) and the four factors of pain(T1) through the multi-group path analysis. When used loneliness(T1) as a mediator, loneliness can fully mediate the temporal relationship between "hostile cognition(T0)"/"suppressive hostility behavior(T0)" and the "pain impact(T1)"/"pain-losing-face(T1)."Discussion: According to cross-sectional and longitudinal analysis, this study finds that hostility factors can be more predictive and influence more aspects of pain in longitudinal analysis. Besides, loneliness substantially affects the relationship between hostility and pain when regarded as a mediator. In conclusion, when chronic pain patients hold higher hostility cognitive thinking in mind or tend to use suppressive hostility behaviors frequently, these hostility presentations would increase loneliness subsequently, making the patient feel shame on themselves, amplifying the physical and psychological impact of pain on patients. By exploring the relationship among hostility, pain, and loneliness in different scopes, this study expands the understanding of the psychosocial context of chronic pain patients in Taiwan, looking forward to helping patients with chronic pain coping and general life adaptation. en_US dc.description.tableofcontents 第一章 緒論 1第二章 文獻回顧 3第一節 慢性疼痛 3第二節 慢性疼痛病人的敵意經驗 11第三節 寂寞感、敵意與慢性疼痛之關係 18第四節 研究架構及假設 25第三章 研究方法 30第一節 研究參與者與研究流程 30第二節 研究工具 31第三節 資料分析 38第四章 研究一結果 41第一節 樣本概述與各變項描述統計 41第二節 量表工具信度檢驗 44第三節 敵意與疼痛程度之同時性關係 45第四節 寂寞感對敵意與疼痛同時性關係之調節效果 52第五章 研究二結果 57第一節 敵意與疼痛程度之時序性關係 57第二節 寂寞感對敵意與疼痛時序性關係之影響 59第六章 討論 68第一節 敵意與疼痛程度之關係統整 68第二節 寂寞感對敵意與疼痛關係之影響概述 72第三節 研究貢獻與應用、限制與未來方向 76參考文獻 80附錄 91一、基本資料與疼痛評估 91二、台灣疼痛評估量表(第三版) 92三、慢性病負向情緒篩檢量表 93四、台灣慢性疼痛病人寂寞量表(初版) 93五、短式華人敵意量表 94 zh_TW dc.format.extent 4704757 bytes - dc.format.mimetype application/pdf - dc.source.uri (資料來源) http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G0107752003 en_US dc.subject (關鍵詞) 慢性疼痛 zh_TW dc.subject (關鍵詞) 敵意 zh_TW dc.subject (關鍵詞) 寂寞感 zh_TW dc.subject (關鍵詞) chronic pain en_US dc.subject (關鍵詞) hostility en_US dc.subject (關鍵詞) loneliness en_US dc.title (題名) 慢性疼痛病人的敵意覺知、寂寞感與疼痛程度之關聯探究 zh_TW dc.title (題名) Exploring the Relationship among Hostility, Loneliness, and Pain Level in Chronic Pain Patients en_US dc.type (資料類型) thesis en_US dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) 吳治勳,林至芃,楊啟正,葉芷圻。(2017)。臨床情境下之全面性疼痛評估:「台灣疼痛評估量表」之發展與心理計量特性建立。取自https://www.grb.gov.tw/search/planDetail?id=11908191林宜美、翁嘉英。(2002)。冠狀動脈心臟病與頭痛的敵意型態比較。中華心理學刊,44(2),211–226。翁嘉英、林宜美、呂碧鴻、陳秀蓉、吳英璋、鄭逸如。(2008)。「短式華人敵意量表」之發展與信效度考驗。測驗學刊,55(3),463–487。 https://doi.org/10.7108/PT.200812.0002陳梓、喻惠丹。(2018)。慢性病負性情緒篩查量表在冠心病患者中的信效度檢驗。中國臨床心理學雜誌,26(4),666–670。Anema, J. R., Schellart, A. J., Cassidy, J. D., Loisel, P., Veerman, T. J., & Van der Beek, A. J. (2009). Can cross country differences in return-to-work after chronic occupational back pain be explained? An exploratory analysis on disability policies in a six country cohort study. Journal of occupational rehabilitation, 19(4), 419-426. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10926-009-9202-3Asmundson, J. G., &Katz, J. (2009). Understanding the co-occurrence of anxiety disorders and chronic pain: state-of-the-art. Depression and Anxiety, 26(10), 888–901. https://doi.org/10.1002/DA.20600Averill, J. R. (1983). Studies on anger and aggression: Implications for theories of emotion. American Psychologist, 38(11), 1145–1160. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.38.11.1145Bair, M. J., Robinson, R. L., Katon, W., &Kroenke, K. (2003). Depression and Pain Comorbidity: A Literature Review. Archives of Internal Medicine, 163(20), 2433–2445. https://doi.org/10.1001/ARCHINTE.163.20.2433Barefoot, J. C. (1992). Developments in the measurement of hostility. In Hostility, coping, & health. (pp. 13–31). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10105-001Berkowitz, L., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2004). Toward an understanding of the determinants of anger. Emotion, 4(2), 107. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.4.2.107Boggero, I. A., Sturgeon, J. A., Arewasikporn, A., Castro, S. A., King, C. D., &Segerstrom, S. C. (2019). Associations of Pain Intensity and Frequency With Loneliness, Hostility, and Social Functioning: Cross-Sectional, Longitudinal, and Within-Person Relationships. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 26(2), 217–229. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-019-09776-5Bruehl, S., Chung, O. Y., Ward, P., Johnson, B., &McCubbin, J. A. (2002). The relationship between resting blood pressure and acute pain sensitivity in healthy normotensives and chronic back pain sufferers: The effects of opioid blockade. Pain, 100(1–2), 191–201. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00295-6Bruehl, S., Liu, X., Burns, J. W., Chont, M., &Jamison, R. N. (2012). Associations between daily chronic pain intensity, daily anger expression, and trait anger expressiveness: An ecological momentary assessment study. Pain, 153(12), 2352–2358. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2012.08.001Burke, A. L., Mathias, J. L., & Denson, L. A. (2015). Psychological functioning of people living with chronic pain: A meta‐analytic review. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 54(3), 345-360. https://doi.org/10.1111/BJC.12078Burns, J. W., Bruehl, S., &Chont, M. (2014). Anger regulation style, anger arousal and acute pain sensitivity: Evidence for an endogenous opioid “triggering” model. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 37(4), 642–653. https://doi.org/10.1007/s-10865-0139511-zBurns, J. W., Bruehl, S., &Quartana, P. J. (2006). Anger management style and hostility among patients with chronic pain: Effects on symptom-specific physiological reactivity during anger- and sadness- recall interviews. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68(5), 786–793. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.psy.0000238211.89198.e4Burns, J. W., Gerhart, J. I., Bruehl, S., Peterson, K. M., Smith, D. A., Porter, L. S., ... & Keefe, F. J. (2015). Anger arousal and behavioral anger regulation in everyday life among patients with chronic low back pain: Relationships to patient pain and function. Health Psychology, 34(5),547. https://doi.org/10.1037/HEA0000091Burns, J. W., Gerhart, J. I., Bruehl, S., Post, K. M., Smith, D. A., Porter, L. S., ... & Keefe, F. J. (2016). Anger arousal and behavioral anger regulation in everyday life among people with chronic low back pain: Relationships with spouse responses and negative affect. Health Psychology, 35(1),29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/hea0000221Burns, J. W., Johnson, B. J., Devine, J., Mahoney, N., &Pawl, R. (1998). Anger management style and the prediction of treatment outcome among male and female chronic pain patients. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(11), 1051–1062. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00080-1Buss, A. H., &Durkee, A. (1957). An inventory for assessing different kinds of hostility. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 21(4), 343.Buss, A. H., &Perry, M. (1992). The Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(3), 452–459. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.452Cacioppo, J. T., &Hawkley, L. C. (2009). Perceived social isolation and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(10), 447–454. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.005Cacioppo, S., Grippo, A. J., London, S., Goossens, L., &Cacioppo, J. T. (2015). Loneliness: Clinical Import and Interventions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 238–249. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615570616Eckhardt, C., Norlander, B., &Deffenbacher, J. (2004). The assessment of anger and hostility: a critical review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 9(1), 17–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1359-1789(02)00116-7Edwards, R. R., Dworkin, R. H., Sullivan, M. D., Turk, D. C., &Wasan, A. D. (2016). The Role of Psychosocial Processes in the Development and Maintenance of Chronic Pain. Journal of Pain, 17(9), T70–T92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2016.01.001Eisenberger, N. I. (2012). The pain of social disconnection: Examining the shared neural underpinnings of physical and social pain. In Nature Reviews Neuroscience (Vol. 13, Issue 6, pp. 421–434). Nature Publishing Group. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3231Eisenberger, N. I., &Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why rejection hurts: A common neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(7), 294–300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.010Engel, G. L. (1977). The need for a new medical model: A challenge for biomedicine. Science, 196(4286), 129–136. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.847460Fernandez, E., &Turk, D. C. (1995). The scope and significance of anger in the experience of chronic pain. In Pain (Vol. 61, Issue 2, pp. 165–175). https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(95)00192-UFillingim, R. B., King, C. D., Ribeiro-Dasilva, M. C., Rahim-Williams, B., &Riley, J. L. (2009). Sex, Gender, and Pain: A Review of Recent Clinical and Experimental Findings. In Journal of Pain (Vol. 10, Issue 5, pp. 447–485). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2008.12.001Gatchel, R. J. (2004). Comorbidity of chronic pain and mental health disorders: the biopsychosocial perspective. American Psychologist, 59(8), 795. https://doi.org/10.1037/00303-066X.59.8.795Gerrits, M. M. J. G., vanMarwijk, H. W. J., vanOppen, P., van derHorst, H., &Penninx, B. W. J. H. (2015). Longitudinal association between pain, and depression and anxiety over four years. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 78(1), 64–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JPSYCHORES.2014.10.011Hemingway, H., &Marmot, M. (1999). Evidence based cardiology: Psychosocial factors in the aetiology and prognosis of coronary heart disease: systematic review of prospective cohort studies. BMJ : British Medical Journal, 318(7196), 1460. https://doi.org/10.1136/BMJ.318.7196.1460Huang, Y. H., Wu, C. H., Chen, H. J., Cheng, Y. R., Hung, F. C., Leung, K. K., Lue, B. H., Chen, C. Y., Chiu, T. Y., &Wu, Y. C. (2017). Quick screening tool for patients with severe negative emotional reactions to chronic illness: Psychometric study of the negative emotions due to chronic illness screening test (NECIS). Family Practice, 35(1), 34–40. https://doi.org/10.1093/fampra/cmx076Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). A short scale for measuring loneliness in large surveys: Results from two population-based studies. Research on aging, 26(6), 655-672. https://doi.org/10.1177/0164027504268574IASP. (2020). IASP Announces Revised Definition of Pain. https://www.iasp-pain.org/publications/iasp-news/iasp-announces-revised-definition-of-pain/?ItemNumber=10475Jamison, R. N., &Virts, K. L. (1990). The influence of family support on chronic pain. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28(4), 283–287. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(90)90079-XJensen, M. P., Moore, M. R., Bockow, T. B., Ehde, D. M., & Engel, J. M. (2011). Psychosocial factors and adjustment to chronic pain in persons with physical disabilities: a systematic review. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation, 92(1), 146-160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2010.09.021Leeuw, M., Goossens, M. E. J. B., Linton, S. J., Crombez, G., Boersma, K., &Vlaeyen, J. W. S. (2007). The fear-avoidance model of musculoskeletal pain: current state of scientific evidence. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 30(1), 77–94. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-006-9085-0Leonard, M. T., Cano, A., &Johansen, A. B. (2006). Chronic Pain in a Couples Context: A Review and Integration of Theoretical Models and Empirical Evidence. The Journal of Pain, 7(6), 377–390. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JPAIN.2006.01.442López-Martínez, A. E., Esteve-Zarazaga, R., &Ramírez-Maestre, C. (2008). Perceived Social Support and Coping Responses Are Independent Variables Explaining Pain Adjustment Among Chronic Pain Patients. Journal of Pain, 9(4), 373–379. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2007.12.002McWilliams, L. A., Cox, B. J., &Enns, M. W. (2003). Mood and anxiety disorders associated with chronic pain: An examination in a nationally representative sample. Pain, 106(1–2), 127–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00301-4Meints, S. M., &Edwards, R. R. (2018). Evaluating psychosocial contributions to chronic pain outcomes. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 87, 168–182. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PNPBP.2018.01.017Melzack, R., Wall, P. D., &others. (1965). Pain Mechanisms: A New Theory: A gate control system modulates sensory input from the skin before it evokes pain perception and response. Science, 150(3699), 971–979. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.150.3699.971Meng, J., Wang, X., Wei, D., &Qiu, J. (2020). State loneliness is associated with emotional hypervigilance in daily life: A network analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 165, 110154. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PAID.2020.110154Mills, S. E. E., Nicolson, K. P., &Smith, B. H. (2019). Chronic pain: a review of its epidemiology and associated factors in population-based studies. British Journal of Anaesthesia, 123(2), e273–e283. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BJA.2019.03.023Moayedi, M., &Davis, K. D. (2013). Theories of pain: From specificity to gate control. Journal of Neurophysiology, 109(1), 5–12. https://doi.org/10.1152/JN.00457.2012Ohayon, M. M., &Schatzberg, A. F. (2010). Chronic pain and major depressive disorder in the general population. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 44(7), 454–461. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JPSYCHIRES.2009.10.013Okifuji, A., Turk, D. C., &Curran, S. L. (1999). Anger in chronic pain: Investigations of anger targets and intensity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 47(1), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(99)00006-9Peplau, L. A., Perlman, D. (1982). Perspectives on loneliness. In Peplau, L. A., Perlman, D. , Loneliness: A sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy , (pp. 1–8.)Qualter, P., Vanhalst, J., Harris, R., VanRoekel, E., Lodder, G., Bangee, M., Maes, M., &Verhagen, M. (2015). Loneliness Across the Life Span. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 250–264. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615568999Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., &Cutrona, C. E. (1980). The revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(3), 472–480. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.39.3.472Rustøen, T., Wahl, A. K., Hanestad, B. R., Lerdal, A., Paul, S., &Miaskowski, C. (2005). Age and the experience of chronic pain: Differences in health and quality of life among younger, middle-aged, and older adults. Clinical Journal of Pain, 21(6), 513–523. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.ajp.0000146217.31780.efSegel-Karpas, D., &Ayalon, L. (2020). Loneliness and hostility in older adults: A cross-lagged model. Psychology and Aging, 35(2), 169. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000417Smith, T. W. (1992). Hostility and health: Current status of a psychosomatic hypothesis. Health Psychology, 11(3), 139–150. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.11.3.139Smith, T. W., Glazer, K., Ruiz, J. M., &Gallo, L. C. (2004). Hostility, Anger, Aggressiveness, and Coronary Heart Disease: An Interpersonal Perspective on Personality, Emotion, and Health. Journal of Personality, 72(6), 1217–1270. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1467-6494.2004.00296.XSmith, T. W., &Weihs, K. (2019). Emotion, Social Relationships, and Physical Health: Concepts, Methods, and Evidence for an Integrative Perspective. Psychosomatic Medicine, 81(8), 681–693. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000739Spielberger, C. D., Jacobs, G., Russell, S., &Crane, R. S. (1983). Assessment of anger: The state-trait anger scale. Advances in Personality Assessment, 2, 161–189.Spielberger, C. D., Johnson, E. H., Jacobs, G. A., Krasner, S. S., Oesterle, S. E., &Worden, T. J. (1986). The anger expression (AX) scale. Tampa, FL: Center for Research in Behavioral Medicine and Community Psychology.Spielberger, C. D., Reheiser, E. C., &Sydeman, S. J. (1995). Measuring the experience, expression, and control of anger. Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric Nursing, 18(3), 207–232. https://doi.org/10.3109/01460869509087271Spithoven, A. W. M., Bijttebier, P., &Goossens, L. (2017). It is all in their mind: A review on information processing bias in lonely individuals. Clinical Psychology Review, 58, 97–114. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CPR.2017.10.003Steingrímsdóttir, Ó. A., Landmark, T., Macfarlane, G. J., &Nielsen, C. S. (2017). Defining chronic pain in epidemiological studies: A systematic review and meta-analysis. In Pain (Vol. 158, Issue 11, pp. 2092–2107). Lippincott Williams and Wilkins. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001009Sturgeon, J. A., &Zautra, A. J. (2016). Social pain and physical pain: shared paths to resilience. In Pain management (Vol. 6, Issue 1, pp. 63–74). Future Science Group. https://doi.org/10.2217/pmt.15.56Treede, R. D., Rief, W., Barke, A., Aziz, Q., Bennett, M. I., Benoliel, R., Cohen, M., Evers, S., Finnerup, N. B., First, M. B., Giamberardino, M. A., Kaasa, S., Korwisi, B., Kosek, E., Lavand’Homme, P., Nicholas, M., Perrot, S., Scholz, J., Schug, S., …Wang, S. J. (2019). Chronic pain as a symptom or a disease: The IASP Classification of Chronic Pain for the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11). Pain, 160(1), 19–27. https://doi.org/10.1097/J.PAIN.0000000000001384Victor, C. R., Scambler, S. J., Bowling, A., &Bond, J. (2005). The prevalence of, and risk factors for, loneliness in later life: a survey of older people in Great Britain. Ageing & Society, 25(6), 357–375. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X04003332Vlaeyen, J. W. S., Kole-Snijders, A. M. J., Boeren, R. G. B., &vanEek, H. (1995). Fear of movement/(re)injury in chronic low back pain and its relation to behavioral performance. Pain, 62(3), 363–372. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(94)00279-NVlaeyen, J. W. S., Kole-Snijders, A. M. J., Rotteveel, A. M., Ruesink, R., &Heuts, P. H. T. G. (1995). The role of fear of movement/(re)injury in pain disability. Journal of Occupational Rehabilitation, 5(4), 235–252. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02109988Vlaeyen, J. W. S., &Linton, S. J. (2000). Fear-avoidance and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: A state of the art. In Pain (Vol. 85, Issue 3, pp. 317–332). No longer published by Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00242-0Weiss, R. S. (1973). Loneliness: The experience of emotional and social isolation. The MIT Press.WHO. (2021). ICD-11 Reference Guide, International Classification of Diseases for Mortality and Morbidity Statistics. http://id.who.int/icd/entity/437815624Wiech, K., & Tracey, I. (2009). The influence of negative emotions on pain: behavioral effects and neural mechanisms. Neuroimage, 47(3), 987-994. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.059 zh_TW dc.identifier.doi (DOI) 10.6814/NCCU202200772 en_US