學術產出-學位論文

文章檢視/開啟

書目匯出

Google ScholarTM

政大圖書館

引文資訊

TAIR相關學術產出

題名 測量陰謀信念之研究:驗證中文版陰謀信念量表
Measuring Belief in Conspiracy Theories: Validation of a Chinese Scale
作者 李京蓁
Li, Jing-Zhen
貢獻者 韓義興
Han, Yi-Hsing
李京蓁
Li, Jing-Zhen
關鍵詞 陰謀論
陰謀信念
陰謀心態
建構效度
量表發展
conspiracy theories
conspiracy beliefs
conspiracy mentality
construct valid
scale development
日期 2024
上傳時間 4-九月-2024 15:05:17 (UTC+8)
摘要 針對陰謀信念的研究持續盛行於二十一世紀,根據 Aaronovitch(2009)提到 陰謀信念是當個人尋求對於事件或某種類型事件的解釋時,即使其他的解釋更有 可能,卻不必要地假設是有隱藏的個人或團體的陰謀所致。Brotherton 等人(2013) 則進一步發展出包括五個面向的一般陰謀信念量表(generic conspiracist beliefs scale, GCB),分別是政府陰謀(government malfeasance conspiracies)、外星人陰 謀(extraterrestrial cover-up conspiracies)、訊息控制陰謀(control of information conspiracies)、個人幸福陰謀(personal well-being conspiracies)和惡意全球陰謀 (malevolent global conspiracies),受到國內外研究採用。 在台灣,常見媒體上以陰謀論解讀爭議事件,並對政治及社會產生不利影響。 然而,有別於前述學者已歸納陰謀信念的主要面向及發展出英文的測量工具,台灣關於陰謀信念的研究,似乎尚缺具有共識的定義及有效的中文測量工具。有鑑 於陰謀論對民主參與及社會持續帶來的負面影響,為使相關研究能聚焦及延展, 本研究借用 Brotherton 等人(2013)的量表,嘗試在台灣發展並驗證具備信度與效度的中文陰謀信念量表。 本研究以問卷調查研究法,經由第一階段(N = 107)的兩次探索性因素分析,同時在第二階段(N = 844)以驗證性因素分析的方式來建構量表的收斂效 度,並加入與陰謀信念相近之陰謀心態問卷(CMQ)、失範、超自然信念、威權 主義、五大人格特質中的開放性、友善性、神經質來檢視區別效度後,結果顯示中文版陰謀信念量表二階單因子測量模型適配度良好且具備信度與效度。
Research on conspiracy beliefs has continued to flourish in the twenty-first century. According to Aaronovitch (2009), conspiracy beliefs arise when individuals seek explanations for events or specific events, even if other explanations are more plausible, by unnecessarily attributing them to hidden conspiracies by individuals or groups. Brotherton et al(2013) further developed the Generic Conspiracist Beliefs Scale (GCB), which includes five dimensions: Government Malfeasance Conspiracies, Extraterrestrial Cover-up Conspiracies, Control of Information Conspiracies, Personal Well-being Conspiracies, and Malevolent Global Conspiracies. This scale has been widely adopted in research both domestically and internationally. In Taiwan, the media frequently uses conspiracy theories to interpret controversial events, resulting in adverse effects on politics and society. Thus, there is a pressing need for a universally accepted definition and an effective measurement tool for conspiracy beliefs. Given the detrimental effects of conspiracy theories on democratic participation and social sustainability, this study is crucial in its aim to develop and validate a reliable and valid Chinese Conspiracy Belief Scale, drawing on the scale developed by Brotherton et al. (2013). This study employed a survey research method. In the first phase, two rounds of exploratory factor analysis (N = 107) were conducted, and in the second phase (N = 844), confirmatory factor analysis was used to establish the convergent validity of the scale. Additionally, the Conspiracy Mentality Questionnaire (CMQ), anomia, belief in the paranormal, authoritarianism, and the Big Five personality traits of openness, agreeableness, and neuroticism were included to examine discriminant validity. The results indicated that the second-order single-factor measurement model of the Chinese version of the Conspiracy Belief Scale exhibited a good fit and demonstrated reliability and validity.
參考文獻 李仁豪、鍾芯瑜(2020)。中文版簡式 [五大人格量表] BFI 的發展。測驗學刊, 67(4),271-299。 莊伯仲(2006)。競選廣告:1996-2004 年台灣與美國總統大選之分析。東吳政治學報,24,213-243。東吳大學,台北市。 須予謙(2023)。新冠肺炎疫情下臺灣的族群集體自戀與陰謀論。臺灣大學正社會科學院政治學系碩士論文研究所。 鄭怡卉(2013)。新聞中的「偽科學」內容分析研究。新聞學研究,116,47-90。 杜兆倫(2018)。謠言分藍綠? 政治傾向與社群媒體謠言傳播之關聯性研究。臺灣大學新聞研究所學位論文,台北市。 胡正光、江素慧(2020)。民粹主義在台灣─ 韓國瑜及其支持者。臺灣國際研究季刊,16(3),155-183。 Abalakina‐Paap, M., Stephan, W. G., Craig, T., & Gregory, W. L. (1999). Beliefs in conspiracies. Political Psychology, 20(3), 637-647. https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00160 Ardèvol‐Abreu, A., Gil de Zuniga, H., & Gámez, E. (2020). The influence of conspiracy beliefs on conventional and unconventional forms of political participation: The mediating role of political efficacy. British Journal of Social Psychology, 59(2), 549-569. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12366 Andrade, G. (2020). Medical conspiracy theories: cognitive science and implications for ethics. Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy, 23, 505-518. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11019-020-09951-6 Allington, D., & Dhavan, N. (2020). The relationship between conspiracy beliefs and compliance with public health guidance about COVID-19. Centre for Countering Digital Hate: London, UK. Adam‐Troian, J., Chayinska, M., Paladino, M. P., Uluğ, Ö. M., Vaes, J., & Wagner‐Egger, P. (2023). Of precarity and conspiracy: Introducing a socio‐functional model of conspiracy beliefs. British journal of social psychology, 62, 136-159. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12597 Butler, L. D., Koopman, C., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1995). The psychological impact of viewing the film" JFK": Emotions, beliefs, and political behavioral intentions. Political psychology, 237-257. https://www.jstor.org/stable/3791831 Bergmann, W. (2008). Anti-Semitic attitudes in Europe: a comparative perspective. Journal of Social Issues, 64, 343–362. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2008.00565.x Brotherton, R., French, C. C., & Pickering, A. D. (2013). Measuring belief in conspiracy theories: The generic conspiracist beliefs scale. Frontiers in psychology, 279. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00279 Bruder, M., Haffke, P., Neave, N., Nouripanah, N., & Imhoff, R. (2013). Measuring individual differences in generic beliefs in conspiracy theories across cultures: Conspiracy Mentality Questionnaire. Frontiers in psychology, 4, 225. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00225 Bizumic, B., & Duckitt, J. (2018). Investigating right wing authoritarianism with a very short authoritarianism scale. https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v6i1.835 Buturoiu, R., Udrea, G., Oprea, D. A., & Corbu, N. (2021). Who believes in conspiracy theories about the COVID-19 pandemic in Romania? An analysis of conspiracy theories believers’ profiles. Societies, 11(4), 138. https://doi.org/10.3390/soc11040138 Converse, P. E., Campbell, A., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1961). Stability and change in 1960: a reinstating election. American Political Science Review, 55(2), 269-280. https://doi.org/10.2307/1952237 Clarke, S. (2002). Conspiracy theories and conspiracy theorizing. Philosophy of the social sciences, 32(2), 131-150. https://doi.org/10.1177/004931032002001 Castanho Silva, B., Vegetti, F., & Littvay, L. (2017). The elite is up to something: Exploring the relation between populism and belief in conspiracy theories. Swiss political science review, 23(4), 423-443. https://doi.org/10.1111/spsr.12270 Cinelli, M., De Francisci Morales, G., Galeazzi, A., Quattrociocchi, W., & Starnini, M. (2021). The echo chamber effect on social media. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(9), e2023301118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2023301118 Christner, C. (2022). Populist attitudes and conspiracy beliefs: Exploring the relation between the latent structures of populist attitudes and conspiracy beliefs. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 10(1), 72-85. https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.7969 Casara, B. G. S., Suitner, C., & Jetten, J. (2022). The impact of economic inequality on conspiracy beliefs. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 98, 104245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2021.104245 De Coninck, D., Frissen, T., Matthijs, K., d’Haenens, L., Lits, G., Champagne-Poirier, O., ... & Généreux, M. (2021). Beliefs in conspiracy theories and misinformation about COVID-19: Comparative perspectives on the role of anxiety, depression and exposure to and trust in information sources. Frontiers in psychology, 12, 646394. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.646394 Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2008). The hidden impact of conspiracy theories: Perceived and actual influence of theories surrounding the death of Princess Diana. The Journal of social psychology, 148(2), 210-222. https://doi.org/10.3200/SOCP.148.2.210-222 Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2011). Does it take one to know one? Endorsement of conspiracy theories is influenced by personal willingness to conspire. British Journal of Social Psychology, 50(3), 544–552. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.2010.02018. Darwin, H., Neave, N., & Holmes, J. (2011). Belief in conspiracy theories. The role of paranormal belief, paranoid ideation and schizotypy. Personality and individual differences, 50(8), 1289-1293. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.027 Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., Callan, M. J., Dawtry, R. J., & Harvey, A. J. (2016). Someone is pulling the strings: Hypersensitive agency detection and belief in conspiracy theories. Thinking & Reasoning, 22(1), 57-77. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2015.1051586 Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., & Cichocka, A. (2017). The psychology of conspiracy theories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(6), 538-542. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417718261 Douglas, K. M., Uscinski, J. E., Sutton, R. M., Cichocka, A., Nefes, T., Ang, C. S., & Deravi, F. (2019). Understanding conspiracy theories. Political psychology, 40, 3-35. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12568 Dyrendal, A., & Jolley, D. (2020). Conspiracy theories in the classroom: Problems and potential solutions. Religions, 11(10), 494. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel11100494 Dyrendal, A., Kennair, L. E. O., & Bendixen, M. (2021). Predictors of belief in conspiracy theory: The role of individual differences in schizotypal traits, paranormal beliefs, social dominance orientation, right wing authoritarianism and conspiracy mentality. Personality and individual differences, 173, 110645. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.110645 Einstein, K. L., & Glick, D. M. (2014). Do I Think BLS Data are BS? The Consequences of Conspiracy Theories. Political Behavior, 37(3), 679–701. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-014-9287-z Enders, A. M., & Smallpage, S. M. (2019). Informational cues, partisan-motivated reasoning, and the manipulation of conspiracy beliefs. Political Communication, 36(1), 83-102. https://doi.org/10.1080/10584609.2018.1493006 Earnshaw, V. A., Eaton, L. A., Kalichman, S. C., Brousseau, N. M., Hill, E. C., & Fox, A. B. (2020). COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs, health behaviors, and policy support. Translational behavioral medicine, 10(4), 850-856. https://doi.org/10.1093/tbm/ibaa090 Enders, A. M., Uscinski, J. E., Seelig, M. I., Klofstad, C. A., Wuchty, S., Funchion, J. R., ... & Stoler, J. (2021). The relationship between social media use and beliefs in conspiracy theories and misinformation. Political behavior, 1-24. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-021-09734-6 Freeman, D., Bentall, R.P. The concomitants of conspiracy concerns. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 52, 595–604 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-017-1354-4 Fernández, M., Bellogín, A., & Cantador, I. (2021). Analysing the effect of recommendation algorithms on the amplification of misinformation. arXiv preprint arXiv:2103.14748. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2103.14748 Farias, J., & Pilati, R. (2023). COVID-19 as an undesirable political issue: Conspiracy beliefs and intolerance of uncertainty predict adhesion to prevention measures. Current Psychology, 42(1), 209-219. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01416-0 Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 15, 731–742. https://doi.org/10.2307/3791630 Grzesiak-Feldman, M., & Ejsmont, A. (2008). Paranoia and Conspiracy Thinking of Jews, Arabs, Germans, and Russians in a Polish Sample. Psychological Reports, 102(3), 884–886. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.102.3.884-886 Garrett, R. K. (2011). Troubling Consequences of Online Political Rumoring. Human Communication Research, 37(2), 255–274. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.14682958.2010.01401.x Goreis, A., & Voracek, M. (2019). A systematic review and meta-analysis of psychological research on conspiracy beliefs: Field characteristics, measurement instruments, and associations with personality traits. Frontiers in psychology, 10, 205. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00205 Green, R., Toribio-Flórez, D., Douglas, K. M., Brunkow, J. W., & Sutton, R. M. (2023). Making an impression: The effects of sharing conspiracy theories. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 104, 104398. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2022.104398 Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., & Anderson, R. E. (2009). Multivariate Data Analysis, 7th, Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Henseler, J., Ringle, C. M., & Sarstedt, M. (2015). A new criterion for assessing discriminant validity in variance-based structural equation modeling. Journal of the academy of marketing science, 43, 115-135. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11747-014-0403-8 Hornsey, M. J., Finlayson, M., Chatwood, G., & Begeny, C. T. (2020). Donald Trump and vaccination: The effect of political identity, conspiracist ideation and presidential tweets on vaccine hesitancy. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 88, 103947. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103947 Hameleers, M. (2021). They are selling themselves out to the enemy! The content and effects of populist conspiracy theories. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 33(1), 38-56. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijpor/edaa004 Imhoff, R., & Bruder, M. (2014). Speaking (un–) truth to power: Conspiracy mentality as a generalised political attitude. European Journal of Personality, 28(1), 25-43. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1930 Imhoff, R., Zimmer, F., Klein, O., António, J. H., Babinska, M., Bangerter, A., ... & Van Prooijen, J. W. (2022). Conspiracy mentality and political orientation across 26 countries. Nature human behaviour, 6(3), 392-403. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01258-7 Johnson, A. A. (2013). Moral Knowledge and its Enemies: Conspiracy and Kingship in Thailand. Anthropological Quarterly, 86(4), 1059–1086. https://doi.org/10.1353/anq.2013.0052 Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (2014a). The Effects of Anti-Vaccine Conspiracy Theories on Vaccination Intentions. PLoS ONE, 9(2), e89177. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0089177 Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (2014b). The social consequences of conspiracism: Exposure to conspiracy theories decreases intentions to engage in politics and to reduce one's carbon footprint. British Journal of Psychology, 105(1), 35-56. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12018 Jennings, W., Stoker, G., Bunting, H., Valgarðsson, V. O., Gaskell, J., Devine, D., ... & Mills, M. C. (2021). Lack of trust, conspiracy beliefs, and social media use predict COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy. Vaccines, 9(6), 593. https://doi.org/10.3390/vaccines9060593 Jamieson, K. H., & Albarracin, D. (2020). The relation between media consumption and misinformation at the outset of the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic in the US. The Harvard Kennedy School Misinformation Review, 1(2), 1-22. https://doi.org/10.37016/mr-2020-012 Jolley, D., Douglas, K. M., Skipper, Y., Thomas, E., & Cookson, D. (2021). Measuring adolescents’ beliefs in conspiracy theories: Development and validation of the Adolescent Conspiracy Beliefs Questionnaire (ACBQ). British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 39(3), 499-520. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12368 Keeley, B. L. (1999). Of conspiracy theories. The journal of Philosophy, 96(3), 109-126. Kata, A. (2010). A postmodern Pandora's box: anti-vaccination misinformation on the Internet. Vaccine, 28(7), 1709-1716. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2009.12.022 Lewandowsky, S., Gignac, G. E., & Oberauer, K. (2013). The role of conspiracist ideation and worldviews in predicting rejection of science. PloS one, 8(10), e75637. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075637 Lobato, E., Mendoza, J., Sims, V., & Chin, M. (2014). Examining the relationship between conspiracy theories, paranormal beliefs, and pseudoscience acceptance among a university population. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(5), 617-625. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3042 Lee, T., & Hosam, C. (2020). Fake news is real: The significance and sources of disbelief in mainstream media in Trump’s America. In Sociological Forum (Vol. 35, pp. 996-1018). https://doi.org/10.1111/socf.12603 McCauley, C., & Jacques, S. (1979). The popularity of conspiracy theories of presidential assassination: A Bayesian analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(5), 637–644. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.37.5.637 Miller, S. (2002). Conspiracy Theories: Public Arguments as Coded Social Critiques: a Rhetorical Analysis of the Twa Flight 800 Conspiracy Theories. Argumentation and Advocacy, 39(1). 40-56. https://doi.org/10.1080/00028533.2002.11821576 Musch, J., & Ehrenberg, K. (2002). Probability misjudgment, cognitive ability, and belief in the paranormal. British Journal of Psychology, 93(2), 169-177. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712602162517 Mocanu, D., Rossi, L., Zhang, Q., Karsai, M., & Quattrociocchi, W. (2015). Collective attention in the age of (mis) information. Computers in Human Behavior, 51, 1198-1204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.01.024 Moulding, R., Nix-Carnell, S., Schnabel, A., Nedeljkovic, M., Burnside, E. E., Lentini, A. F., & Mehzabin, N. (2016). Better the devil you know than a world you don't? Intolerance of uncertainty and worldview explanations for belief in conspiracy theories. Personality and individual differences, 98, 345-354. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.060 Niemi, R. G., Craig, S. C., & Mattei, F. (1991). Measuring internal political efficacy in the 1988 National Election Study. American Political Science Review, 85(4), 1407-1413. https://doi.org/10.2307/1963953 Nguyen, N. P., Yan, G., Thai, M. T., & Eidenbenz, S. (2012). Containment of misinformation spread in online social networks. In Proceedings of the 4th Annual ACM Web Science Conference (pp. 213-222). https://doi.org/10.1145/2380718.2380746 Nyhan, B., Dickinson, F., Dudding, S., Dylgjeri, E., Neiley, E., Pullerits, C., … Walmsley, C. (2016). Classified or Coverup? The Effect of Redactions on Conspiracy Theory Beliefs. Journal of Experimental Political Science, 3(02), 109–123. https://doi.org /10.1017/xps.2015.21 Oliver, J. E., & Wood, T. J. (2014). Conspiracy theories and the paranoid style (s) of mass opinion. American journal of political science, 58(4), 952-966. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12084 Popper, K. R. (1963). Science as falsification. Conjectures and refutations, 1(1963), 33-39. Park, C. L. (2010). Making sense of the meaning literature: an integrative review of meaning making and its effects on adjustment to stressful life events. Psychological bulletin, 136(2), 257. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018301 Popoli, G., & Longus, A. (2021). Gender differences and the five facets of conspiracy theory. International Journal of Psychological Studies, 13(3), 64-69. https://doi.org/10.5539/ijps.v13n3p64 Palmer, L., & Waisbord, S. (2022). Polarisation and the network harassment of science journalists. Impact of Social Sciences Blog. Pertwee, E., Simas, C., & Larson, H. J. (2022). An epidemic of uncertainty: rumors, conspiracy theories and vaccine hesitancy. Nature medicine, 28(3), 456-459. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41591-022-01728-z Ross, M. W., Essien, E. J., & Torres, I. (2006). Conspiracy beliefs about the origin of HIV/AIDS in four racial/ethnic groups. Journal of acquired immune deficiency syndromes (1999), 41(3), 342. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.qai.0000209897.59384.52 Ratner, B. (2009). The correlation coefficient: Its values range between+ 1/− 1, or do they?. Journal of targeting, measurement and analysis for marketing, 17(2), 139-142. https://doi.org/10.1057/jt.2009.5 Romer, D., & Jamieson, K. H. (2020). Conspiracy theories as barriers to controlling the spread of COVID-19 in the US. Social science & medicine, 263, 113356. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.113356 Ruiz, J. B., & Bell, R. A. (2021). Predictors of intention to vaccinate against COVID-19: Results of a nationwide survey. Vaccine, 39(7), 1080–1086. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2021.01.010 Salmon, D. A., Moulton, L. H., Omer, S. B., DeHart, M. P., Stokley, S., & Halsey, N. A. (2005). Factors associated with refusal of childhood vaccines among parents of school-aged children: a case-control study. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine, 159(5), 470-476. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.159.5.470 Stempel, C., Hargrove, T., & Stempel III, G. H. (2007). Media use, social structure, and belief in 9/11 conspiracy theories. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 84(2), 353-372. https://doi.org/10.1177/107769900708400210 Sunstein, C. R., & Vermeule, A. (2009). Conspiracy theories: Causes and cures. Journal of political philosophy, 17(2), 202-227. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9760.2008.00325.x. Swami, V., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2010). Unanswered questions: A preliminary investigation of personality and individual difference predictors of 9/11 conspiracist beliefs. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 749–761. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1583 Swami, V., Coles, R., Stieger, S., Pietschnig, J., Furnham, A., Rehim, S., & Voracek, M. (2011). Conspiracist ideation in Britain and Austria: Evidence of a monological belief system and associations between individual psychological differences and real-world and fictitious conspiracy theories. British Journal of Psychology, 102(3), 443–463. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02004.x Swami, V. (2012). Social psychological origins of conspiracy theories: The case of the Jewish conspiracy theory in Malaysia. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, Article 280. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00280 Swami, V., & Furnham, A. (2012). Examining conspiracist beliefs about the disappearance of Amelia Earhart. The Journal of General Psychology, 139(4), 244-259. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2012.697932 Sallam, M., Dababseh, D., Yaseen, A., Al-Haidar, A., Ababneh, N. A., Bakri, F. G., & Mahafzah, A. (2020). Conspiracy beliefs are associated with lower knowledge and higher anxiety levels regarding COVID-19 among students at the University of Jordan. International journal of environmental research and public health, 17(14), 4915. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17144915 Sternisko, A., Cichocka, A., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2020). The dark side of social movements: Social identity, non-conformity, and the lure of conspiracy theories. Current opinion in psychology, 35, 1-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.02.007 Sallam, M., Dababseh, D., Eid, H., Al-Mahzoum, K., Al-Haidar, A., Taim, D., ... & Mahafzah, A. (2021). High rates of COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy and its association with conspiracy beliefs: a study in Jordan and Kuwait among other Arab countries. Vaccines, 9(1), 42. https://doi.org/10.3390/vaccines9010042 Sanchez, C., & Dunning, D. (2021). The anti‐scientists bias: The role of feelings about scientists in COVID‐19 attitudes and behaviors. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 51(4), 461-473. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12748 Rullo, M., & Telesca, G. (2023). Consequences of conspiracy theories on political efficacy and political participation. In Understanding radicalization in everyday life (pp. 259-276). McGraw-Hill Education. Thorburn, S., & Bogart, L. M. (2005). Conspiracy beliefs about birth control: barriers to pregnancy prevention among African Americans of reproductive age. Health Education & Behavior, 32(4), 474-487. https://doi.org/10.1177/1090198105276220 Uscinski, J. E., & Parent, J. M. (2014). American conspiracy theories. Oxford University Press. Uscinski, J. E., Douglas, K., & Lewandowsky, S. (2017). Climate change conspiracy theories. In Oxford research encyclopedia of climate science. https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228620.013.328 Van Prooijen, J. W., & Jostmann, N. B. (2013). Belief in conspiracy theories: The influence of uncertainty and perceived morality. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43(1), 109-115. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.1922 Van Prooijen, J.-W., & Acker, M. (2015). The Influence of Control on Belief in Conspiracy Theories: Conceptual and Applied Extensions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29(5), 753–761. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3161 Van Prooijen, J.-W., Krouwel, A. P. M., & Pollet, T. V. (2015). Political Extremism Predicts Belief in Conspiracy Theories. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6(5), 570–578. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550614567356 Van Prooijen, J. W., & de Vries, R. E. (2016). Organizational conspiracy beliefs: Implications for leadership styles and employee outcomes. Journal of business and psychology, 31, 479-491. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-015-9428-3 Van Prooijen, J. W., & Douglas, K. M. (2017). Conspiracy theories as part of history: The role of societal crisis situations. Memory studies, 10(3), 323-333. https://doi.org/10.1177/1750698017701615 Whitson, J. A., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception. science, 322(5898), 115-117. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1159845 Wood, M. J., Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2012). Dead and alive: Beliefs in contradictory conspiracy theories. Social psychological and personality science, 3(6), 767-773. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550611434786 Wood, M. J., & Gray, D. (2019). Right-wing authoritarianism as a predictor of pro-establishment versus anti-establishment conspiracy theories. Personality and Individual Differences, 138, 163-166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.09.036 Warner, B. R., & Neville-Shepard, R. (2014). Echoes of a conspiracy: Birthers, truthers, and the cultivation of extremism. Communication Quarterly, 62(1), 1-17. https://doi.org/10.1080/01463373.2013.822407 Zakrisson, I. (2005). Construction of a short version of the Right-Wing Authoritarianism (RWA) scale. Personality and individual differences, 39(5), 863-872. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.02.026 Zhou, Y., & Pinkleton, B. E. (2012). Modeling the effects of political information source use and online expression on young adults’ political efficacy. Mass Communication and Society, 15(6), 813-830. https://doi.org/10.1080/15205436.2011.622064
描述 碩士
國立政治大學
傳播學院傳播碩士學位學程
110464022
資料來源 http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G0110464022
資料類型 thesis
dc.contributor.advisor 韓義興zh_TW
dc.contributor.advisor Han, Yi-Hsingen_US
dc.contributor.author (作者) 李京蓁zh_TW
dc.contributor.author (作者) Li, Jing-Zhenen_US
dc.creator (作者) 李京蓁zh_TW
dc.creator (作者) Li, Jing-Zhenen_US
dc.date (日期) 2024en_US
dc.date.accessioned 4-九月-2024 15:05:17 (UTC+8)-
dc.date.available 4-九月-2024 15:05:17 (UTC+8)-
dc.date.issued (上傳時間) 4-九月-2024 15:05:17 (UTC+8)-
dc.identifier (其他 識別碼) G0110464022en_US
dc.identifier.uri (URI) https://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/153406-
dc.description (描述) 碩士zh_TW
dc.description (描述) 國立政治大學zh_TW
dc.description (描述) 傳播學院傳播碩士學位學程zh_TW
dc.description (描述) 110464022zh_TW
dc.description.abstract (摘要) 針對陰謀信念的研究持續盛行於二十一世紀,根據 Aaronovitch(2009)提到 陰謀信念是當個人尋求對於事件或某種類型事件的解釋時,即使其他的解釋更有 可能,卻不必要地假設是有隱藏的個人或團體的陰謀所致。Brotherton 等人(2013) 則進一步發展出包括五個面向的一般陰謀信念量表(generic conspiracist beliefs scale, GCB),分別是政府陰謀(government malfeasance conspiracies)、外星人陰 謀(extraterrestrial cover-up conspiracies)、訊息控制陰謀(control of information conspiracies)、個人幸福陰謀(personal well-being conspiracies)和惡意全球陰謀 (malevolent global conspiracies),受到國內外研究採用。 在台灣,常見媒體上以陰謀論解讀爭議事件,並對政治及社會產生不利影響。 然而,有別於前述學者已歸納陰謀信念的主要面向及發展出英文的測量工具,台灣關於陰謀信念的研究,似乎尚缺具有共識的定義及有效的中文測量工具。有鑑 於陰謀論對民主參與及社會持續帶來的負面影響,為使相關研究能聚焦及延展, 本研究借用 Brotherton 等人(2013)的量表,嘗試在台灣發展並驗證具備信度與效度的中文陰謀信念量表。 本研究以問卷調查研究法,經由第一階段(N = 107)的兩次探索性因素分析,同時在第二階段(N = 844)以驗證性因素分析的方式來建構量表的收斂效 度,並加入與陰謀信念相近之陰謀心態問卷(CMQ)、失範、超自然信念、威權 主義、五大人格特質中的開放性、友善性、神經質來檢視區別效度後,結果顯示中文版陰謀信念量表二階單因子測量模型適配度良好且具備信度與效度。zh_TW
dc.description.abstract (摘要) Research on conspiracy beliefs has continued to flourish in the twenty-first century. According to Aaronovitch (2009), conspiracy beliefs arise when individuals seek explanations for events or specific events, even if other explanations are more plausible, by unnecessarily attributing them to hidden conspiracies by individuals or groups. Brotherton et al(2013) further developed the Generic Conspiracist Beliefs Scale (GCB), which includes five dimensions: Government Malfeasance Conspiracies, Extraterrestrial Cover-up Conspiracies, Control of Information Conspiracies, Personal Well-being Conspiracies, and Malevolent Global Conspiracies. This scale has been widely adopted in research both domestically and internationally. In Taiwan, the media frequently uses conspiracy theories to interpret controversial events, resulting in adverse effects on politics and society. Thus, there is a pressing need for a universally accepted definition and an effective measurement tool for conspiracy beliefs. Given the detrimental effects of conspiracy theories on democratic participation and social sustainability, this study is crucial in its aim to develop and validate a reliable and valid Chinese Conspiracy Belief Scale, drawing on the scale developed by Brotherton et al. (2013). This study employed a survey research method. In the first phase, two rounds of exploratory factor analysis (N = 107) were conducted, and in the second phase (N = 844), confirmatory factor analysis was used to establish the convergent validity of the scale. Additionally, the Conspiracy Mentality Questionnaire (CMQ), anomia, belief in the paranormal, authoritarianism, and the Big Five personality traits of openness, agreeableness, and neuroticism were included to examine discriminant validity. The results indicated that the second-order single-factor measurement model of the Chinese version of the Conspiracy Belief Scale exhibited a good fit and demonstrated reliability and validity.en_US
dc.description.tableofcontents 第一章 緒論 1 第一節 研究背景 1 第二節 研究目的 6 第三節 研究問題 7 第二章 文獻回顧 10 第一節 陰謀論 10 第二節 陰謀信念 19 第三節 台灣陰謀論的研究 25 第四節 中文版陰謀信念量表的效度建構 31 第三章 研究方法 37 第一節 第一階段問卷調查及探索性因素分析 37 第二節 建立及驗證陰謀信念量表 52 第三節 資料分析階段說明 61 第四章 研究結果 64 第一節 替代模型檢定與驗證性因素分析 64 第二節 建構效度 74 第三節 人口變項與陰謀信念得分比較 81 第五章 結論 93 第一節 研究發現與建議 93 第二節 研究限制與建議 100 參考文獻 103 一、 中文部分 103 二、 英文部分 103 附 錄 114 附錄一:前測|衡量個人對重大事件的普遍信念差異之問卷 114 附錄二:中文版陰謀信念EFA轉軸後的因素負荷量 119 附錄三:正式問卷|衡量個人對重大事件的普遍信念差異之問卷 122 附錄四:修正後二階單因子模型的參數估計結果 129zh_TW
dc.format.extent 2517867 bytes-
dc.format.mimetype application/pdf-
dc.source.uri (資料來源) http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G0110464022en_US
dc.subject (關鍵詞) 陰謀論zh_TW
dc.subject (關鍵詞) 陰謀信念zh_TW
dc.subject (關鍵詞) 陰謀心態zh_TW
dc.subject (關鍵詞) 建構效度zh_TW
dc.subject (關鍵詞) 量表發展zh_TW
dc.subject (關鍵詞) conspiracy theoriesen_US
dc.subject (關鍵詞) conspiracy beliefsen_US
dc.subject (關鍵詞) conspiracy mentalityen_US
dc.subject (關鍵詞) construct validen_US
dc.subject (關鍵詞) scale developmenten_US
dc.title (題名) 測量陰謀信念之研究:驗證中文版陰謀信念量表zh_TW
dc.title (題名) Measuring Belief in Conspiracy Theories: Validation of a Chinese Scaleen_US
dc.type (資料類型) thesisen_US
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) 李仁豪、鍾芯瑜(2020)。中文版簡式 [五大人格量表] BFI 的發展。測驗學刊, 67(4),271-299。 莊伯仲(2006)。競選廣告:1996-2004 年台灣與美國總統大選之分析。東吳政治學報,24,213-243。東吳大學,台北市。 須予謙(2023)。新冠肺炎疫情下臺灣的族群集體自戀與陰謀論。臺灣大學正社會科學院政治學系碩士論文研究所。 鄭怡卉(2013)。新聞中的「偽科學」內容分析研究。新聞學研究,116,47-90。 杜兆倫(2018)。謠言分藍綠? 政治傾向與社群媒體謠言傳播之關聯性研究。臺灣大學新聞研究所學位論文,台北市。 胡正光、江素慧(2020)。民粹主義在台灣─ 韓國瑜及其支持者。臺灣國際研究季刊,16(3),155-183。 Abalakina‐Paap, M., Stephan, W. G., Craig, T., & Gregory, W. L. (1999). Beliefs in conspiracies. Political Psychology, 20(3), 637-647. https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00160 Ardèvol‐Abreu, A., Gil de Zuniga, H., & Gámez, E. (2020). The influence of conspiracy beliefs on conventional and unconventional forms of political participation: The mediating role of political efficacy. British Journal of Social Psychology, 59(2), 549-569. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12366 Andrade, G. (2020). Medical conspiracy theories: cognitive science and implications for ethics. Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy, 23, 505-518. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11019-020-09951-6 Allington, D., & Dhavan, N. (2020). The relationship between conspiracy beliefs and compliance with public health guidance about COVID-19. Centre for Countering Digital Hate: London, UK. Adam‐Troian, J., Chayinska, M., Paladino, M. P., Uluğ, Ö. M., Vaes, J., & Wagner‐Egger, P. (2023). Of precarity and conspiracy: Introducing a socio‐functional model of conspiracy beliefs. British journal of social psychology, 62, 136-159. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12597 Butler, L. D., Koopman, C., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1995). The psychological impact of viewing the film" JFK": Emotions, beliefs, and political behavioral intentions. Political psychology, 237-257. https://www.jstor.org/stable/3791831 Bergmann, W. (2008). Anti-Semitic attitudes in Europe: a comparative perspective. Journal of Social Issues, 64, 343–362. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2008.00565.x Brotherton, R., French, C. C., & Pickering, A. D. (2013). Measuring belief in conspiracy theories: The generic conspiracist beliefs scale. Frontiers in psychology, 279. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00279 Bruder, M., Haffke, P., Neave, N., Nouripanah, N., & Imhoff, R. (2013). Measuring individual differences in generic beliefs in conspiracy theories across cultures: Conspiracy Mentality Questionnaire. Frontiers in psychology, 4, 225. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00225 Bizumic, B., & Duckitt, J. (2018). Investigating right wing authoritarianism with a very short authoritarianism scale. https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v6i1.835 Buturoiu, R., Udrea, G., Oprea, D. A., & Corbu, N. (2021). Who believes in conspiracy theories about the COVID-19 pandemic in Romania? An analysis of conspiracy theories believers’ profiles. Societies, 11(4), 138. https://doi.org/10.3390/soc11040138 Converse, P. E., Campbell, A., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1961). Stability and change in 1960: a reinstating election. American Political Science Review, 55(2), 269-280. https://doi.org/10.2307/1952237 Clarke, S. (2002). Conspiracy theories and conspiracy theorizing. Philosophy of the social sciences, 32(2), 131-150. https://doi.org/10.1177/004931032002001 Castanho Silva, B., Vegetti, F., & Littvay, L. (2017). The elite is up to something: Exploring the relation between populism and belief in conspiracy theories. Swiss political science review, 23(4), 423-443. https://doi.org/10.1111/spsr.12270 Cinelli, M., De Francisci Morales, G., Galeazzi, A., Quattrociocchi, W., & Starnini, M. (2021). The echo chamber effect on social media. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(9), e2023301118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2023301118 Christner, C. (2022). Populist attitudes and conspiracy beliefs: Exploring the relation between the latent structures of populist attitudes and conspiracy beliefs. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 10(1), 72-85. https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.7969 Casara, B. G. S., Suitner, C., & Jetten, J. (2022). The impact of economic inequality on conspiracy beliefs. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 98, 104245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2021.104245 De Coninck, D., Frissen, T., Matthijs, K., d’Haenens, L., Lits, G., Champagne-Poirier, O., ... & Généreux, M. (2021). Beliefs in conspiracy theories and misinformation about COVID-19: Comparative perspectives on the role of anxiety, depression and exposure to and trust in information sources. Frontiers in psychology, 12, 646394. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.646394 Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2008). The hidden impact of conspiracy theories: Perceived and actual influence of theories surrounding the death of Princess Diana. The Journal of social psychology, 148(2), 210-222. https://doi.org/10.3200/SOCP.148.2.210-222 Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2011). Does it take one to know one? Endorsement of conspiracy theories is influenced by personal willingness to conspire. British Journal of Social Psychology, 50(3), 544–552. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.2010.02018. Darwin, H., Neave, N., & Holmes, J. (2011). Belief in conspiracy theories. The role of paranormal belief, paranoid ideation and schizotypy. Personality and individual differences, 50(8), 1289-1293. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.027 Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., Callan, M. J., Dawtry, R. J., & Harvey, A. J. (2016). Someone is pulling the strings: Hypersensitive agency detection and belief in conspiracy theories. Thinking & Reasoning, 22(1), 57-77. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2015.1051586 Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., & Cichocka, A. (2017). The psychology of conspiracy theories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(6), 538-542. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417718261 Douglas, K. M., Uscinski, J. E., Sutton, R. M., Cichocka, A., Nefes, T., Ang, C. S., & Deravi, F. (2019). Understanding conspiracy theories. Political psychology, 40, 3-35. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12568 Dyrendal, A., & Jolley, D. (2020). Conspiracy theories in the classroom: Problems and potential solutions. Religions, 11(10), 494. https://doi.org/10.3390/rel11100494 Dyrendal, A., Kennair, L. E. O., & Bendixen, M. (2021). Predictors of belief in conspiracy theory: The role of individual differences in schizotypal traits, paranormal beliefs, social dominance orientation, right wing authoritarianism and conspiracy mentality. Personality and individual differences, 173, 110645. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.110645 Einstein, K. L., & Glick, D. M. (2014). Do I Think BLS Data are BS? The Consequences of Conspiracy Theories. Political Behavior, 37(3), 679–701. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-014-9287-z Enders, A. M., & Smallpage, S. M. (2019). Informational cues, partisan-motivated reasoning, and the manipulation of conspiracy beliefs. Political Communication, 36(1), 83-102. https://doi.org/10.1080/10584609.2018.1493006 Earnshaw, V. A., Eaton, L. A., Kalichman, S. C., Brousseau, N. M., Hill, E. C., & Fox, A. B. (2020). COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs, health behaviors, and policy support. Translational behavioral medicine, 10(4), 850-856. https://doi.org/10.1093/tbm/ibaa090 Enders, A. M., Uscinski, J. E., Seelig, M. I., Klofstad, C. A., Wuchty, S., Funchion, J. R., ... & Stoler, J. (2021). The relationship between social media use and beliefs in conspiracy theories and misinformation. Political behavior, 1-24. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-021-09734-6 Freeman, D., Bentall, R.P. The concomitants of conspiracy concerns. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 52, 595–604 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-017-1354-4 Fernández, M., Bellogín, A., & Cantador, I. (2021). Analysing the effect of recommendation algorithms on the amplification of misinformation. arXiv preprint arXiv:2103.14748. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2103.14748 Farias, J., & Pilati, R. (2023). COVID-19 as an undesirable political issue: Conspiracy beliefs and intolerance of uncertainty predict adhesion to prevention measures. Current Psychology, 42(1), 209-219. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01416-0 Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 15, 731–742. https://doi.org/10.2307/3791630 Grzesiak-Feldman, M., & Ejsmont, A. (2008). Paranoia and Conspiracy Thinking of Jews, Arabs, Germans, and Russians in a Polish Sample. Psychological Reports, 102(3), 884–886. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.102.3.884-886 Garrett, R. K. (2011). Troubling Consequences of Online Political Rumoring. Human Communication Research, 37(2), 255–274. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.14682958.2010.01401.x Goreis, A., & Voracek, M. (2019). A systematic review and meta-analysis of psychological research on conspiracy beliefs: Field characteristics, measurement instruments, and associations with personality traits. Frontiers in psychology, 10, 205. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00205 Green, R., Toribio-Flórez, D., Douglas, K. M., Brunkow, J. W., & Sutton, R. M. (2023). Making an impression: The effects of sharing conspiracy theories. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 104, 104398. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2022.104398 Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., & Anderson, R. E. (2009). Multivariate Data Analysis, 7th, Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Henseler, J., Ringle, C. M., & Sarstedt, M. (2015). A new criterion for assessing discriminant validity in variance-based structural equation modeling. Journal of the academy of marketing science, 43, 115-135. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11747-014-0403-8 Hornsey, M. J., Finlayson, M., Chatwood, G., & Begeny, C. T. (2020). Donald Trump and vaccination: The effect of political identity, conspiracist ideation and presidential tweets on vaccine hesitancy. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 88, 103947. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103947 Hameleers, M. (2021). They are selling themselves out to the enemy! The content and effects of populist conspiracy theories. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 33(1), 38-56. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijpor/edaa004 Imhoff, R., & Bruder, M. (2014). Speaking (un–) truth to power: Conspiracy mentality as a generalised political attitude. European Journal of Personality, 28(1), 25-43. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1930 Imhoff, R., Zimmer, F., Klein, O., António, J. H., Babinska, M., Bangerter, A., ... & Van Prooijen, J. W. (2022). Conspiracy mentality and political orientation across 26 countries. Nature human behaviour, 6(3), 392-403. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01258-7 Johnson, A. A. (2013). Moral Knowledge and its Enemies: Conspiracy and Kingship in Thailand. Anthropological Quarterly, 86(4), 1059–1086. https://doi.org/10.1353/anq.2013.0052 Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (2014a). The Effects of Anti-Vaccine Conspiracy Theories on Vaccination Intentions. PLoS ONE, 9(2), e89177. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0089177 Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (2014b). The social consequences of conspiracism: Exposure to conspiracy theories decreases intentions to engage in politics and to reduce one's carbon footprint. British Journal of Psychology, 105(1), 35-56. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12018 Jennings, W., Stoker, G., Bunting, H., Valgarðsson, V. O., Gaskell, J., Devine, D., ... & Mills, M. C. (2021). Lack of trust, conspiracy beliefs, and social media use predict COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy. Vaccines, 9(6), 593. https://doi.org/10.3390/vaccines9060593 Jamieson, K. H., & Albarracin, D. (2020). The relation between media consumption and misinformation at the outset of the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic in the US. The Harvard Kennedy School Misinformation Review, 1(2), 1-22. https://doi.org/10.37016/mr-2020-012 Jolley, D., Douglas, K. M., Skipper, Y., Thomas, E., & Cookson, D. (2021). Measuring adolescents’ beliefs in conspiracy theories: Development and validation of the Adolescent Conspiracy Beliefs Questionnaire (ACBQ). British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 39(3), 499-520. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12368 Keeley, B. L. (1999). Of conspiracy theories. The journal of Philosophy, 96(3), 109-126. Kata, A. (2010). A postmodern Pandora's box: anti-vaccination misinformation on the Internet. Vaccine, 28(7), 1709-1716. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2009.12.022 Lewandowsky, S., Gignac, G. E., & Oberauer, K. (2013). The role of conspiracist ideation and worldviews in predicting rejection of science. PloS one, 8(10), e75637. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075637 Lobato, E., Mendoza, J., Sims, V., & Chin, M. (2014). Examining the relationship between conspiracy theories, paranormal beliefs, and pseudoscience acceptance among a university population. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(5), 617-625. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3042 Lee, T., & Hosam, C. (2020). Fake news is real: The significance and sources of disbelief in mainstream media in Trump’s America. In Sociological Forum (Vol. 35, pp. 996-1018). https://doi.org/10.1111/socf.12603 McCauley, C., & Jacques, S. (1979). The popularity of conspiracy theories of presidential assassination: A Bayesian analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(5), 637–644. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.37.5.637 Miller, S. (2002). Conspiracy Theories: Public Arguments as Coded Social Critiques: a Rhetorical Analysis of the Twa Flight 800 Conspiracy Theories. Argumentation and Advocacy, 39(1). 40-56. https://doi.org/10.1080/00028533.2002.11821576 Musch, J., & Ehrenberg, K. (2002). Probability misjudgment, cognitive ability, and belief in the paranormal. British Journal of Psychology, 93(2), 169-177. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712602162517 Mocanu, D., Rossi, L., Zhang, Q., Karsai, M., & Quattrociocchi, W. (2015). Collective attention in the age of (mis) information. Computers in Human Behavior, 51, 1198-1204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.01.024 Moulding, R., Nix-Carnell, S., Schnabel, A., Nedeljkovic, M., Burnside, E. E., Lentini, A. F., & Mehzabin, N. (2016). Better the devil you know than a world you don't? Intolerance of uncertainty and worldview explanations for belief in conspiracy theories. Personality and individual differences, 98, 345-354. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.060 Niemi, R. G., Craig, S. C., & Mattei, F. (1991). Measuring internal political efficacy in the 1988 National Election Study. American Political Science Review, 85(4), 1407-1413. https://doi.org/10.2307/1963953 Nguyen, N. P., Yan, G., Thai, M. T., & Eidenbenz, S. (2012). Containment of misinformation spread in online social networks. In Proceedings of the 4th Annual ACM Web Science Conference (pp. 213-222). https://doi.org/10.1145/2380718.2380746 Nyhan, B., Dickinson, F., Dudding, S., Dylgjeri, E., Neiley, E., Pullerits, C., … Walmsley, C. (2016). Classified or Coverup? The Effect of Redactions on Conspiracy Theory Beliefs. Journal of Experimental Political Science, 3(02), 109–123. https://doi.org /10.1017/xps.2015.21 Oliver, J. E., & Wood, T. J. (2014). Conspiracy theories and the paranoid style (s) of mass opinion. American journal of political science, 58(4), 952-966. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12084 Popper, K. R. (1963). Science as falsification. Conjectures and refutations, 1(1963), 33-39. Park, C. L. (2010). Making sense of the meaning literature: an integrative review of meaning making and its effects on adjustment to stressful life events. Psychological bulletin, 136(2), 257. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018301 Popoli, G., & Longus, A. (2021). Gender differences and the five facets of conspiracy theory. International Journal of Psychological Studies, 13(3), 64-69. https://doi.org/10.5539/ijps.v13n3p64 Palmer, L., & Waisbord, S. (2022). Polarisation and the network harassment of science journalists. Impact of Social Sciences Blog. Pertwee, E., Simas, C., & Larson, H. J. (2022). An epidemic of uncertainty: rumors, conspiracy theories and vaccine hesitancy. Nature medicine, 28(3), 456-459. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41591-022-01728-z Ross, M. W., Essien, E. J., & Torres, I. (2006). Conspiracy beliefs about the origin of HIV/AIDS in four racial/ethnic groups. Journal of acquired immune deficiency syndromes (1999), 41(3), 342. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.qai.0000209897.59384.52 Ratner, B. (2009). The correlation coefficient: Its values range between+ 1/− 1, or do they?. Journal of targeting, measurement and analysis for marketing, 17(2), 139-142. https://doi.org/10.1057/jt.2009.5 Romer, D., & Jamieson, K. H. (2020). Conspiracy theories as barriers to controlling the spread of COVID-19 in the US. Social science & medicine, 263, 113356. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.113356 Ruiz, J. B., & Bell, R. A. (2021). Predictors of intention to vaccinate against COVID-19: Results of a nationwide survey. Vaccine, 39(7), 1080–1086. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vaccine.2021.01.010 Salmon, D. A., Moulton, L. H., Omer, S. B., DeHart, M. P., Stokley, S., & Halsey, N. A. (2005). Factors associated with refusal of childhood vaccines among parents of school-aged children: a case-control study. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine, 159(5), 470-476. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.159.5.470 Stempel, C., Hargrove, T., & Stempel III, G. H. (2007). Media use, social structure, and belief in 9/11 conspiracy theories. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 84(2), 353-372. https://doi.org/10.1177/107769900708400210 Sunstein, C. R., & Vermeule, A. (2009). Conspiracy theories: Causes and cures. Journal of political philosophy, 17(2), 202-227. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9760.2008.00325.x. Swami, V., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2010). Unanswered questions: A preliminary investigation of personality and individual difference predictors of 9/11 conspiracist beliefs. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 749–761. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1583 Swami, V., Coles, R., Stieger, S., Pietschnig, J., Furnham, A., Rehim, S., & Voracek, M. (2011). Conspiracist ideation in Britain and Austria: Evidence of a monological belief system and associations between individual psychological differences and real-world and fictitious conspiracy theories. British Journal of Psychology, 102(3), 443–463. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02004.x Swami, V. (2012). Social psychological origins of conspiracy theories: The case of the Jewish conspiracy theory in Malaysia. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, Article 280. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00280 Swami, V., & Furnham, A. (2012). Examining conspiracist beliefs about the disappearance of Amelia Earhart. The Journal of General Psychology, 139(4), 244-259. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2012.697932 Sallam, M., Dababseh, D., Yaseen, A., Al-Haidar, A., Ababneh, N. A., Bakri, F. G., & Mahafzah, A. (2020). Conspiracy beliefs are associated with lower knowledge and higher anxiety levels regarding COVID-19 among students at the University of Jordan. International journal of environmental research and public health, 17(14), 4915. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17144915 Sternisko, A., Cichocka, A., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2020). The dark side of social movements: Social identity, non-conformity, and the lure of conspiracy theories. Current opinion in psychology, 35, 1-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.02.007 Sallam, M., Dababseh, D., Eid, H., Al-Mahzoum, K., Al-Haidar, A., Taim, D., ... & Mahafzah, A. (2021). High rates of COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy and its association with conspiracy beliefs: a study in Jordan and Kuwait among other Arab countries. Vaccines, 9(1), 42. https://doi.org/10.3390/vaccines9010042 Sanchez, C., & Dunning, D. (2021). The anti‐scientists bias: The role of feelings about scientists in COVID‐19 attitudes and behaviors. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 51(4), 461-473. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12748 Rullo, M., & Telesca, G. (2023). Consequences of conspiracy theories on political efficacy and political participation. In Understanding radicalization in everyday life (pp. 259-276). McGraw-Hill Education. Thorburn, S., & Bogart, L. M. (2005). Conspiracy beliefs about birth control: barriers to pregnancy prevention among African Americans of reproductive age. Health Education & Behavior, 32(4), 474-487. https://doi.org/10.1177/1090198105276220 Uscinski, J. E., & Parent, J. M. (2014). American conspiracy theories. Oxford University Press. Uscinski, J. E., Douglas, K., & Lewandowsky, S. (2017). Climate change conspiracy theories. In Oxford research encyclopedia of climate science. https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228620.013.328 Van Prooijen, J. W., & Jostmann, N. B. (2013). Belief in conspiracy theories: The influence of uncertainty and perceived morality. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43(1), 109-115. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.1922 Van Prooijen, J.-W., & Acker, M. (2015). The Influence of Control on Belief in Conspiracy Theories: Conceptual and Applied Extensions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29(5), 753–761. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3161 Van Prooijen, J.-W., Krouwel, A. P. M., & Pollet, T. V. (2015). Political Extremism Predicts Belief in Conspiracy Theories. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6(5), 570–578. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550614567356 Van Prooijen, J. W., & de Vries, R. E. (2016). Organizational conspiracy beliefs: Implications for leadership styles and employee outcomes. Journal of business and psychology, 31, 479-491. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-015-9428-3 Van Prooijen, J. W., & Douglas, K. M. (2017). Conspiracy theories as part of history: The role of societal crisis situations. Memory studies, 10(3), 323-333. https://doi.org/10.1177/1750698017701615 Whitson, J. A., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception. science, 322(5898), 115-117. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1159845 Wood, M. J., Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2012). Dead and alive: Beliefs in contradictory conspiracy theories. Social psychological and personality science, 3(6), 767-773. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550611434786 Wood, M. J., & Gray, D. (2019). Right-wing authoritarianism as a predictor of pro-establishment versus anti-establishment conspiracy theories. Personality and Individual Differences, 138, 163-166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.09.036 Warner, B. R., & Neville-Shepard, R. (2014). Echoes of a conspiracy: Birthers, truthers, and the cultivation of extremism. Communication Quarterly, 62(1), 1-17. https://doi.org/10.1080/01463373.2013.822407 Zakrisson, I. (2005). Construction of a short version of the Right-Wing Authoritarianism (RWA) scale. Personality and individual differences, 39(5), 863-872. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.02.026 Zhou, Y., & Pinkleton, B. E. (2012). Modeling the effects of political information source use and online expression on young adults’ political efficacy. Mass Communication and Society, 15(6), 813-830. https://doi.org/10.1080/15205436.2011.622064zh_TW