Publications-Theses

題名 不同教學介入對幼兒知識表徵的影響-以幼兒科學問題解決歷程為例
作者 丘嘉慧
Chiu, Chia Hui
貢獻者 柯華葳
丘嘉慧
Chiu, Chia Hui
關鍵詞 幼兒科學學習
幼兒問題解決
幼兒問題解決遷移
知識表徵層次
science learning of young children
the problem-solving of young children
transfer of young children
the level of knowledge representation
日期 2007
上傳時間 19-Sep-2009 12:42:37 (UTC+8)
摘要 本研究主要目的在探討不同年齡幼兒解決需同時考量兩個因素的科學問題及遷移的表現,並探討不同教學介入對幼兒解決科學問題表現及遷移表現的影響。本研究共分二個研究,使用「幼兒認知作業」,分別在研究一探討128位,研究二探討286位3至6歲幼兒的表現。研究結果顯示,幼兒的蒐集訊息、分析整理訊息與解決問題表現間有因果關係。年齡愈大的幼兒在各個歷程的表現愈好。若是作業內容與幼兒生活經驗相關,4歲幼兒在行為上可以表現出同時考量兩個因素解決問題。解決不同概念的問題,幼兒有不同的表現。6歲幼兒無法表現需同時考量兩個因素問題的遷移。幼兒在解決需同時考量兩個因素問題時,行為及語言知識表徵層次有六個層次(層次0至層次5)。各種教學介入對層次1幼兒沒有影響。當幼兒處於層次3時,教學介入有影響效果,透過語言的說明可以幫助此時的幼兒提升表徵層次,但這些教學介入效果不足以讓幼兒達到層次5,也無法影響至幼兒的遷移表現上。
The purposes of this study were to investigate young children’s ability to resolve problems with two dimensions and transfer, and the influences of instructional interventions on resolving these problems and transferring. There were 128 and 286 3 to 6-year-old young children in study 1 and study 2 respectively. Young children’s cognitive task was used. Results revealed the processes of searching information and analyzing information were related to the problem-solving. 4-year-old children could resolve the problems with two dimensions, when the problems were familiar. There was domain-specific knowledge on problem-solving. 6-year-old children could not transfer two dimensions to new and similar conditions. There were 6 levels knowledge representations of resolving problems with two dimensions in this study. The instructional intervention of explaining improved the level of children with level 3 to level 4, but not to level 5. And there were not effects of instructional interventions on transferring.
參考文獻 王幸雯(2000)兒童速度概念發展之研究。國立台灣師範大學家政教育研究所碩士論文。
白玉玲(2002)幼兒類比推理能力之研究年齡、經驗、關係量的影響。台北市立師範學院國民教育研究所碩士論文。
周淑惠(1997)幼兒自然科學經驗-教材教法。台北:心理出版社。
柯華葳(2007)幼托整合後幼兒園教保活動綱要及能力指標研究-認知領域。教育部。
張麗芬(1992)幼兒類比推理能力之研究。政治大學教育所博士論文。
陳淑敏(1996)從社會互動看皮亞傑與維高斯基的理論及其對幼教之啟示。發表於皮亞傑與維高斯基的對話研討會。臺北市立師範學院。
盧瑞青(2004)幼兒物質概念之研究。國立台東大學幼兒教育學系碩士論文。
鍾靜、鄧玉芬與鄭淑珍(2003)學童生活中時間概念支出探研究。國立台北師範學院學報,16,1-38。
Baillargeon, R. (1986). Representing the existence and location of hidden objects: Object permanence in 6- and 8-month-old infants. Cognition, 23, 21-42.
Bjorklund, D. J., Muir-Broaddus, J. E., & Schneider, W. (1990). The role of knowledge in the development of strategies. In D. F. Bjorklund (Ed.), Children’s strategies. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bredekamp, S., & Copple, C. (1997). (Eds.). Developmentally appropriate practice in early childhood programs. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children.
Brown, A. L. (1990). Domain-specific principles affect learning and transfer in children. Cognitive Science, 14, 107-133.
Brown, A. L., & Kane, M. J. (1988). Preschool children can learn to transfer: Learning to learn and learning by example. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 493-523.
Brown, A. L., Kane, M. J., & Long, C. (1989). Analogical transfer in young children: Analogies as tools for communication and exposition. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 3, 275-293.
Brunner, J. S. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.
Carey, S. Spelke, E. S. (1994). Domain specific knowledge and conceptual change. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cogition and culture. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Case, R., Sandieson, K., & Dennis, S. (1986). Two cognitive-developmental approaches to the design of remedial instruction. Cognitive Development, 1, 293-333.
Chouinard, M. M. (2007). Preschooler’ question. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 72, 1-111.
Dai, D. Y. & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.). (2004). Motivation, Emotion, and Cognition: Integrative Perspectives on Intellectual Development and Functioning (The Educational Psychology Series). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Fabricius, W. (1988). The development of forward search planning in preschoolers. Child Development, 59, 1473-1488.
Freeman, K. E., (1996). Analogical reasoning in 2-year-olds: A comparison of formal and problem-solving paradigms. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Minnesota.
French, L. (2004). Science as the center of a coherent, integrated early childhood curriculum. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 138-149.
Gardner, W. & Rogoff, B. (1990). Children’s deliberateness of planning according to task circumstances. Developmental Psychology, 26, 480-487.
Gauvain, M. (1992). Social influences on the development of planning in advance and during action. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 15, 377-398.
Gelman, S. A. & Wellman, H. M. (1991). Insides and essences: Early understands of the non-obvious. Cognition, 38, 213-244.
Gentner, D. (1983). Structure-mapping: A theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science, 7, 155-170.
Gentner, D., & Toupin, C. (1986). Systematicity and similarity in the development of an analogy. Cognitive Science, 10, 277-300.
Ginsburg, H. P. (1992). Piaget and education: The contributions and limits of genetic epistemology. In L. M. Smith (Ed.), Jean Piaget: Critical assessments (Vol. 3, pp. 354-370). NY: Routledge.
Glassman, M. (1999). All things being equal: the two roads of Piaget and Vygotsky. In P; Lloyd, & C. Fernyhough (Eds.), Lev Vygotsky: Critical assessments (pp. 282-338). NY: Routledge.
Goswami, U. (1998). Cognition in children. UK: Psychology Press.
Goswami, U., & Brown, A. (1989). Melting Chocolate and melting snowmen: Analogical reasoning and causal relations. Cognition, 35, 69-95.
Goswami, U., & Brown, A. (1990). Higher-order structure and relational reasoning: Contrasting analogical and thematic relations. Cognition, 36, 207-226.
Hadjigeorgiou, Y. (2001). The role of wonder and “romance” in early childhood science education. International Journal of Early Years Education, 9, 63-69.
Halford, G. S. (1993). Children’s understanding: The development of mental models. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Halford, G. S., & Andrews, G., Dalton, C., Boag, C., & Zielinski, T. (2002). Young children’s performance on the balance scale: The influence of relational complexity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 81, 417-445.
Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., & Phillips, S. (1998). Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 803-831.
Holyoak, K. J. (1985). The pragmatics of analogical transfer, In G. H. Gower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 19, pp.59-87). NY: Academic Press.
Holyoak, K., Junn, E., & Billman, D. (1984). Development of analogical problem solving skills. Child Development, 55, 2042-2055.
Hudson, J. A., Shapiro, L. R., & Sosa, B. B. (1995). Planning in the real world: Preschool children’s scripts and plans for familiar events. Child Development, 66, 948-998.
Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1992). LISREL: A Guide to the Program and Applications, 3rd edition, Chicago: Scientific Software International, Inc.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1994). Précis of beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Science, 17, 693-745.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1997). Beyond modularity: a developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge: The MIT Press.
Kister, M. C., & Patterson, C. J. (1980). Children’s conceptions of the causes of illness: Understanding of contagion and use of immanent justice. Child Development, 51, 839-846.
Klahr, D. (1985). Solving problems with ambiguous subgoal ordering: Preschoolers’ performance. Child Development, 56, 940-952.
Kreitler, S. & Kreitler, H. (1987). Conceptions and processes of planning: The developmental perspective. In S. L. Friedman, D. K. Scholnick, & R. R. Cocking (Eds.), Blueprints for thinking: The role of planning in cognitive development. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Lewis, M., Alessandri, S. M., & Sullivan, M. W. (1990). Violation of expectancy, loss of control, and anger expressions in young infants. Developmental Psychology, 26, 745-751.
Mandler, J. M. (1983). Structural invariants in development. In L. S. Liben (Ed.), Piaget and the foundations of knowledge (pp. 97-124). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Miller, K. F. & Paredes, D. R. (1996). On the shoulders of giants: Cultural tools and mathematical development. In R. Sternberg & T. Ben-Zeev (Eds.), The nature of mathematical thinking. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Mosier, C. E. & Rogoff, B. (1994). Infants’ instrumental use of their mothers to achieve their goals. Child Development, 65, 70-79.
Normandeau, S., Larivée, S., Roulin, J., & Longeot, R. (2001). The balance-scale dilemma : Either the subject or the experimenter muddles through. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 150, 237-249.
Perkins, D. N., & Salomon, G.. (1992). Transfer of learning. International Encyclopedia of Education (2nd). Oxford: Pergamon Press.
Piaget, J. (1954). The origins of intelligence in children. NY: International Universities Press.
Piaget, J. (1972). The principles of genetic epistemology. NY: Routledge.
Piaget, J. (1973). To understand is to invent: The future of education. NY: Grossman Publishers.
Pine, K. J, & Messer, D. J. (1998). Group collaboration effects and the explicitness of children’s knowledge. Cognitive Development, 13, 109-126.
Pine, K. J, & Messer, D. J. (2000). The effects of explaining another’s actions on children’s implicit theories of balance. Cognition and Instruction, 18, 37-54.
Pine, K. J., & Messer, D. (2003). The development of representations as children learn about balancing. British journal of developmental psychology, 21, 285-301.
Pine, K. J., Lufkin, N., & Messer, D. (2004). More gestures than answers: Children learning about balance. Developmental Psychology, 40, 1059-1067.
Pitt, R. (1983). Development of a general problem-solving schema in adolescence and early adulthood. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 547-584.
Samarapungavan, A., Vosniadou, S. & Brewer, W. F. (1996). Mental models of the earth, sun, and moon: Indian children’s cosmologies. Cognitive Development, 11, 491-521.
Saxe, G. B.(2004). Practices of quantification from a socio-cultural perspective. In A. Demetriou & A. Raftopoulos (Eds.), Cognitive developmental change: Theories, models, and measurement (pp. 241-263). NY: Cambridge University Press.
Saxe, G. B., Gearhart, M., & Guberman, S. R. (1984). The social organization of early number development. In B. Rogoff & J. V. Wertsch (Eds.), New direction for child development (Vol. 23, pp. 19-30). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Saxe, G. B., Gearhart, M., & Guberman, S. R. (1987). Social processes in early number development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 52, 1-160.
Schultz, T. R., Fisher, G. W., Pratt, C. C., & Rulf, S. (1986). Selection of causal rules. Child Development, 57, 143-152.
Siegler, R. S. (1976). Three aspects of cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 481-520.
Siegler, R. S. (1981). Developmental sequences within and between concepts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 46, 1-84.
Siegler, R. S. (1995). How does change occur: A microgenetic study of number conservation? Cognitive Psychology, 28, 225-273.
Siegler, R. S. (1996). Emerging minds: The process of change in children’s thinking. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Siegler, R. S., & Chen, Z. (2002). Developmental of rules and strategies: Balancing the old and the new. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 81, 446-457.
Siegler, R. S., & Jenkins, E. A. (1989). How children discover new strategies. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Siegler, R. S., & McGilly, K. (1989). Strategy choices in children’s time-telling. In I. Levin & D. Zakay (Eds.), Time and human cognition: A life span perspective. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.
Siegler, R. S., & Shrager, J. (1984). Strategy choices in addition and subtraction: How do children know what to do? In C. Sophian (Ed.), Origins of cognitive skills (pp. 229-293). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Solomon, G. E. A., & Cassimatis, N. L. (1999). On facts and conceptual systems: Young children’s integration of their understandings of germs and contagion. Developmental Psychology, 35, 113-126.
Starkey, P., & Cooper, R. (1995). The development of subitizing in young children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19, 399–420.
Steffler, D. J. (2001). Implicit cognition and spelling development. Developmental Review, 21, 168-204.
Surber, C. F., & Gzesh, S. M. (1984). Reversible operations in the balance scale task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 36, 273-310.
Thorndike, E. L. (1913). Educational psychology (Vol. 2). New York: Teachers College, Columbia University.
Tobin, K., Tippins, D. J., & Gallard, A. J. (1994). Research on instructional strategies for teaching science. In G. L. Gabet (ed.), Handbook of research on science teaching and learning (pp. 45-93). NJ: Macmillan.
Tudge, J. R. H. & Winterhoff, P. A. (1999). Vygotsky, Piaget, and Bandura: Perspectives on the relations between the social world and cognitive development. In P. Lloyd, & C. Fernyhough (Eds.), Lev Vygotsky: Critical assessments (v.1) (pp. 311-338). NY: Routledge.
Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1992). Mental models of the earth: a study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 535-586.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1982). Om Barnets Psykiske Udvikling [ On the child’s psychic development]. Copenhage: Nyt Nordisk.
Wikdening, F. (1982). Children’s knowledge about time, distance and velocity interrelations. In W. J. Friedman (Ed.), The developmental psychology of time (pp. 87-112). New York: Academic Press.
Willats, P. (1990). Development of problem solving strategies in infancy. In D. F. Bjorklund (Ed.), Children’s strategies. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Willats, P., & Fabricius, W. (1993). The origin of forward search planning in infancy. Paper presented to the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans, LA.
Wood, D. J. & Middleton, D. J. (1975). A study of assisted problem solving. British Journal of Psychology, 66, 181-191.
Woolley, J. D. (2006). Verbal-behavioral dissociations in development. Child Development, 77, 1539-1553.
Wynn, K. (1992). Children’s acquisition of the number words and the counting system. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 220-251.
描述 博士
國立政治大學
教育研究所
91152515
96
資料來源 http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G0911525151
資料類型 thesis
dc.contributor.advisor 柯華葳zh_TW
dc.contributor.author (Authors) 丘嘉慧zh_TW
dc.contributor.author (Authors) Chiu, Chia Huien_US
dc.creator (作者) 丘嘉慧zh_TW
dc.creator (作者) Chiu, Chia Huien_US
dc.date (日期) 2007en_US
dc.date.accessioned 19-Sep-2009 12:42:37 (UTC+8)-
dc.date.available 19-Sep-2009 12:42:37 (UTC+8)-
dc.date.issued (上傳時間) 19-Sep-2009 12:42:37 (UTC+8)-
dc.identifier (Other Identifiers) G0911525151en_US
dc.identifier.uri (URI) https://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/37212-
dc.description (描述) 博士zh_TW
dc.description (描述) 國立政治大學zh_TW
dc.description (描述) 教育研究所zh_TW
dc.description (描述) 91152515zh_TW
dc.description (描述) 96zh_TW
dc.description.abstract (摘要) 本研究主要目的在探討不同年齡幼兒解決需同時考量兩個因素的科學問題及遷移的表現,並探討不同教學介入對幼兒解決科學問題表現及遷移表現的影響。本研究共分二個研究,使用「幼兒認知作業」,分別在研究一探討128位,研究二探討286位3至6歲幼兒的表現。研究結果顯示,幼兒的蒐集訊息、分析整理訊息與解決問題表現間有因果關係。年齡愈大的幼兒在各個歷程的表現愈好。若是作業內容與幼兒生活經驗相關,4歲幼兒在行為上可以表現出同時考量兩個因素解決問題。解決不同概念的問題,幼兒有不同的表現。6歲幼兒無法表現需同時考量兩個因素問題的遷移。幼兒在解決需同時考量兩個因素問題時,行為及語言知識表徵層次有六個層次(層次0至層次5)。各種教學介入對層次1幼兒沒有影響。當幼兒處於層次3時,教學介入有影響效果,透過語言的說明可以幫助此時的幼兒提升表徵層次,但這些教學介入效果不足以讓幼兒達到層次5,也無法影響至幼兒的遷移表現上。zh_TW
dc.description.abstract (摘要) The purposes of this study were to investigate young children’s ability to resolve problems with two dimensions and transfer, and the influences of instructional interventions on resolving these problems and transferring. There were 128 and 286 3 to 6-year-old young children in study 1 and study 2 respectively. Young children’s cognitive task was used. Results revealed the processes of searching information and analyzing information were related to the problem-solving. 4-year-old children could resolve the problems with two dimensions, when the problems were familiar. There was domain-specific knowledge on problem-solving. 6-year-old children could not transfer two dimensions to new and similar conditions. There were 6 levels knowledge representations of resolving problems with two dimensions in this study. The instructional intervention of explaining improved the level of children with level 3 to level 4, but not to level 5. And there were not effects of instructional interventions on transferring.en_US
dc.description.tableofcontents 第一章 緒論………………………………………………………………………1
第一節 研究動機及重要性………………………………………………………1
第二節 研究目的…………………………………………………………………9
第二章 文獻探討 ………………………………………………………………11
第一節 幼兒的認知發展 ………………………………………………………11
第二節 幼兒的知識表徵 ………………………………………………………18
第三節 幼兒問題解決能力 ……………………………………………………26
第四節 幼兒解決問題能力的遷移 ……………………………………………37
第三章 研究一 …………………………………………………………………42
方法…………………………………………………………………42
結果…………………………………………………………………45
討論…………………………………………………………………66
第四章 研究二…………………………………………………………………69
方法…………………………………………………………………72
結果…………………………………………………………………80
討論 ………………………………………………………………106
第五章 綜合討論………………………………………………………………110
第一節 研究結果與討論………………………………………………………110
第二節 建議與未來研究方向…………………………………………………117
參考文獻 …………………………………………………………………………120
附錄一:研究一故事內容 ………………………………………………………129
附錄二:指導語 …………………………………………………………………132
附錄三:研究二故事內容 ………………………………………………………133
附錄四:研究二指導語 …………………………………………………………135
zh_TW
dc.format.extent 96154 bytes-
dc.format.extent 147890 bytes-
dc.format.extent 381845 bytes-
dc.format.extent 281404 bytes-
dc.format.extent 464766 bytes-
dc.format.extent 179554 bytes-
dc.format.extent 130255 bytes-
dc.format.extent 166844 bytes-
dc.format.mimetype application/pdf-
dc.format.mimetype application/pdf-
dc.format.mimetype application/pdf-
dc.format.mimetype application/pdf-
dc.format.mimetype application/pdf-
dc.format.mimetype application/pdf-
dc.format.mimetype application/pdf-
dc.format.mimetype application/pdf-
dc.language.iso en_US-
dc.source.uri (資料來源) http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G0911525151en_US
dc.subject (關鍵詞) 幼兒科學學習zh_TW
dc.subject (關鍵詞) 幼兒問題解決zh_TW
dc.subject (關鍵詞) 幼兒問題解決遷移zh_TW
dc.subject (關鍵詞) 知識表徵層次zh_TW
dc.subject (關鍵詞) science learning of young childrenen_US
dc.subject (關鍵詞) the problem-solving of young childrenen_US
dc.subject (關鍵詞) transfer of young childrenen_US
dc.subject (關鍵詞) the level of knowledge representationen_US
dc.title (題名) 不同教學介入對幼兒知識表徵的影響-以幼兒科學問題解決歷程為例zh_TW
dc.type (資料類型) thesisen
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) 王幸雯(2000)兒童速度概念發展之研究。國立台灣師範大學家政教育研究所碩士論文。zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) 白玉玲(2002)幼兒類比推理能力之研究年齡、經驗、關係量的影響。台北市立師範學院國民教育研究所碩士論文。zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) 周淑惠(1997)幼兒自然科學經驗-教材教法。台北:心理出版社。zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) 柯華葳(2007)幼托整合後幼兒園教保活動綱要及能力指標研究-認知領域。教育部。zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) 張麗芬(1992)幼兒類比推理能力之研究。政治大學教育所博士論文。zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) 陳淑敏(1996)從社會互動看皮亞傑與維高斯基的理論及其對幼教之啟示。發表於皮亞傑與維高斯基的對話研討會。臺北市立師範學院。zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) 盧瑞青(2004)幼兒物質概念之研究。國立台東大學幼兒教育學系碩士論文。zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) 鍾靜、鄧玉芬與鄭淑珍(2003)學童生活中時間概念支出探研究。國立台北師範學院學報,16,1-38。zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Baillargeon, R. (1986). Representing the existence and location of hidden objects: Object permanence in 6- and 8-month-old infants. Cognition, 23, 21-42.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Bjorklund, D. J., Muir-Broaddus, J. E., & Schneider, W. (1990). The role of knowledge in the development of strategies. In D. F. Bjorklund (Ed.), Children’s strategies. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Bredekamp, S., & Copple, C. (1997). (Eds.). Developmentally appropriate practice in early childhood programs. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Brown, A. L. (1990). Domain-specific principles affect learning and transfer in children. Cognitive Science, 14, 107-133.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Brown, A. L., & Kane, M. J. (1988). Preschool children can learn to transfer: Learning to learn and learning by example. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 493-523.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Brown, A. L., Kane, M. J., & Long, C. (1989). Analogical transfer in young children: Analogies as tools for communication and exposition. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 3, 275-293.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Brunner, J. S. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Carey, S. Spelke, E. S. (1994). Domain specific knowledge and conceptual change. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cogition and culture. New York: Cambridge University Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Case, R., Sandieson, K., & Dennis, S. (1986). Two cognitive-developmental approaches to the design of remedial instruction. Cognitive Development, 1, 293-333.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Chouinard, M. M. (2007). Preschooler’ question. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 72, 1-111.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Dai, D. Y. & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.). (2004). Motivation, Emotion, and Cognition: Integrative Perspectives on Intellectual Development and Functioning (The Educational Psychology Series). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Fabricius, W. (1988). The development of forward search planning in preschoolers. Child Development, 59, 1473-1488.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Freeman, K. E., (1996). Analogical reasoning in 2-year-olds: A comparison of formal and problem-solving paradigms. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Minnesota.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) French, L. (2004). Science as the center of a coherent, integrated early childhood curriculum. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 138-149.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Gardner, W. & Rogoff, B. (1990). Children’s deliberateness of planning according to task circumstances. Developmental Psychology, 26, 480-487.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Gauvain, M. (1992). Social influences on the development of planning in advance and during action. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 15, 377-398.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Gelman, S. A. & Wellman, H. M. (1991). Insides and essences: Early understands of the non-obvious. Cognition, 38, 213-244.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Gentner, D. (1983). Structure-mapping: A theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science, 7, 155-170.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Gentner, D., & Toupin, C. (1986). Systematicity and similarity in the development of an analogy. Cognitive Science, 10, 277-300.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Ginsburg, H. P. (1992). Piaget and education: The contributions and limits of genetic epistemology. In L. M. Smith (Ed.), Jean Piaget: Critical assessments (Vol. 3, pp. 354-370). NY: Routledge.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Glassman, M. (1999). All things being equal: the two roads of Piaget and Vygotsky. In P; Lloyd, & C. Fernyhough (Eds.), Lev Vygotsky: Critical assessments (pp. 282-338). NY: Routledge.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Goswami, U. (1998). Cognition in children. UK: Psychology Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Goswami, U., & Brown, A. (1989). Melting Chocolate and melting snowmen: Analogical reasoning and causal relations. Cognition, 35, 69-95.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Goswami, U., & Brown, A. (1990). Higher-order structure and relational reasoning: Contrasting analogical and thematic relations. Cognition, 36, 207-226.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Hadjigeorgiou, Y. (2001). The role of wonder and “romance” in early childhood science education. International Journal of Early Years Education, 9, 63-69.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Halford, G. S. (1993). Children’s understanding: The development of mental models. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Halford, G. S., & Andrews, G., Dalton, C., Boag, C., & Zielinski, T. (2002). Young children’s performance on the balance scale: The influence of relational complexity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 81, 417-445.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., & Phillips, S. (1998). Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 803-831.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Holyoak, K. J. (1985). The pragmatics of analogical transfer, In G. H. Gower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 19, pp.59-87). NY: Academic Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Holyoak, K., Junn, E., & Billman, D. (1984). Development of analogical problem solving skills. Child Development, 55, 2042-2055.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Hudson, J. A., Shapiro, L. R., & Sosa, B. B. (1995). Planning in the real world: Preschool children’s scripts and plans for familiar events. Child Development, 66, 948-998.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1992). LISREL: A Guide to the Program and Applications, 3rd edition, Chicago: Scientific Software International, Inc.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1994). Précis of beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Science, 17, 693-745.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1997). Beyond modularity: a developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge: The MIT Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Kister, M. C., & Patterson, C. J. (1980). Children’s conceptions of the causes of illness: Understanding of contagion and use of immanent justice. Child Development, 51, 839-846.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Klahr, D. (1985). Solving problems with ambiguous subgoal ordering: Preschoolers’ performance. Child Development, 56, 940-952.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Kreitler, S. & Kreitler, H. (1987). Conceptions and processes of planning: The developmental perspective. In S. L. Friedman, D. K. Scholnick, & R. R. Cocking (Eds.), Blueprints for thinking: The role of planning in cognitive development. New York: Cambridge University Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Lewis, M., Alessandri, S. M., & Sullivan, M. W. (1990). Violation of expectancy, loss of control, and anger expressions in young infants. Developmental Psychology, 26, 745-751.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Mandler, J. M. (1983). Structural invariants in development. In L. S. Liben (Ed.), Piaget and the foundations of knowledge (pp. 97-124). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associateszh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Miller, K. F. & Paredes, D. R. (1996). On the shoulders of giants: Cultural tools and mathematical development. In R. Sternberg & T. Ben-Zeev (Eds.), The nature of mathematical thinking. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Mosier, C. E. & Rogoff, B. (1994). Infants’ instrumental use of their mothers to achieve their goals. Child Development, 65, 70-79.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Normandeau, S., Larivée, S., Roulin, J., & Longeot, R. (2001). The balance-scale dilemma : Either the subject or the experimenter muddles through. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 150, 237-249.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Perkins, D. N., & Salomon, G.. (1992). Transfer of learning. International Encyclopedia of Education (2nd). Oxford: Pergamon Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Piaget, J. (1954). The origins of intelligence in children. NY: International Universities Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Piaget, J. (1972). The principles of genetic epistemology. NY: Routledge.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Piaget, J. (1973). To understand is to invent: The future of education. NY: Grossman Publishers.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Pine, K. J, & Messer, D. J. (1998). Group collaboration effects and the explicitness of children’s knowledge. Cognitive Development, 13, 109-126.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Pine, K. J, & Messer, D. J. (2000). The effects of explaining another’s actions on children’s implicit theories of balance. Cognition and Instruction, 18, 37-54.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Pine, K. J., & Messer, D. (2003). The development of representations as children learn about balancing. British journal of developmental psychology, 21, 285-301.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Pine, K. J., Lufkin, N., & Messer, D. (2004). More gestures than answers: Children learning about balance. Developmental Psychology, 40, 1059-1067.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Pitt, R. (1983). Development of a general problem-solving schema in adolescence and early adulthood. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 547-584.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Samarapungavan, A., Vosniadou, S. & Brewer, W. F. (1996). Mental models of the earth, sun, and moon: Indian children’s cosmologies. Cognitive Development, 11, 491-521.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Saxe, G. B.(2004). Practices of quantification from a socio-cultural perspective. In A. Demetriou & A. Raftopoulos (Eds.), Cognitive developmental change: Theories, models, and measurement (pp. 241-263). NY: Cambridge University Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Saxe, G. B., Gearhart, M., & Guberman, S. R. (1984). The social organization of early number development. In B. Rogoff & J. V. Wertsch (Eds.), New direction for child development (Vol. 23, pp. 19-30). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Saxe, G. B., Gearhart, M., & Guberman, S. R. (1987). Social processes in early number development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 52, 1-160.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Schultz, T. R., Fisher, G. W., Pratt, C. C., & Rulf, S. (1986). Selection of causal rules. Child Development, 57, 143-152.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Siegler, R. S. (1976). Three aspects of cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 481-520.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Siegler, R. S. (1981). Developmental sequences within and between concepts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 46, 1-84.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Siegler, R. S. (1995). How does change occur: A microgenetic study of number conservation? Cognitive Psychology, 28, 225-273.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Siegler, R. S. (1996). Emerging minds: The process of change in children’s thinking. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Siegler, R. S., & Chen, Z. (2002). Developmental of rules and strategies: Balancing the old and the new. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 81, 446-457.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Siegler, R. S., & Jenkins, E. A. (1989). How children discover new strategies. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Siegler, R. S., & McGilly, K. (1989). Strategy choices in children’s time-telling. In I. Levin & D. Zakay (Eds.), Time and human cognition: A life span perspective. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Siegler, R. S., & Shrager, J. (1984). Strategy choices in addition and subtraction: How do children know what to do? In C. Sophian (Ed.), Origins of cognitive skills (pp. 229-293). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Solomon, G. E. A., & Cassimatis, N. L. (1999). On facts and conceptual systems: Young children’s integration of their understandings of germs and contagion. Developmental Psychology, 35, 113-126.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Starkey, P., & Cooper, R. (1995). The development of subitizing in young children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19, 399–420.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Steffler, D. J. (2001). Implicit cognition and spelling development. Developmental Review, 21, 168-204.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Surber, C. F., & Gzesh, S. M. (1984). Reversible operations in the balance scale task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 36, 273-310.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Thorndike, E. L. (1913). Educational psychology (Vol. 2). New York: Teachers College, Columbia University.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Tobin, K., Tippins, D. J., & Gallard, A. J. (1994). Research on instructional strategies for teaching science. In G. L. Gabet (ed.), Handbook of research on science teaching and learning (pp. 45-93). NJ: Macmillan.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Tudge, J. R. H. & Winterhoff, P. A. (1999). Vygotsky, Piaget, and Bandura: Perspectives on the relations between the social world and cognitive development. In P. Lloyd, & C. Fernyhough (Eds.), Lev Vygotsky: Critical assessments (v.1) (pp. 311-338). NY: Routledge.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1992). Mental models of the earth: a study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 535-586.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Vygotsky, L. S. (1982). Om Barnets Psykiske Udvikling [ On the child’s psychic development]. Copenhage: Nyt Nordisk.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Wikdening, F. (1982). Children’s knowledge about time, distance and velocity interrelations. In W. J. Friedman (Ed.), The developmental psychology of time (pp. 87-112). New York: Academic Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Willats, P. (1990). Development of problem solving strategies in infancy. In D. F. Bjorklund (Ed.), Children’s strategies. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Willats, P., & Fabricius, W. (1993). The origin of forward search planning in infancy. Paper presented to the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans, LA.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Wood, D. J. & Middleton, D. J. (1975). A study of assisted problem solving. British Journal of Psychology, 66, 181-191.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Woolley, J. D. (2006). Verbal-behavioral dissociations in development. Child Development, 77, 1539-1553.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Wynn, K. (1992). Children’s acquisition of the number words and the counting system. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 220-251.zh_TW