dc.contributor.advisor | 顏乃欣 | zh_TW |
dc.contributor.advisor | Yen, Nai-Shing | en_US |
dc.contributor.author (Authors) | 廖楷民 | zh_TW |
dc.contributor.author (Authors) | Liao, Kai-min | en_US |
dc.creator (作者) | 廖楷民 | zh_TW |
dc.creator (作者) | Liao, Kai-min | en_US |
dc.date (日期) | 2006 | en_US |
dc.date.accessioned | 17-Sep-2009 13:21:56 (UTC+8) | - |
dc.date.available | 17-Sep-2009 13:21:56 (UTC+8) | - |
dc.date.issued (上傳時間) | 17-Sep-2009 13:21:56 (UTC+8) | - |
dc.identifier (Other Identifiers) | G0927520041 | en_US |
dc.identifier.uri (URI) | https://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/32537 | - |
dc.description (描述) | 碩士 | zh_TW |
dc.description (描述) | 國立政治大學 | zh_TW |
dc.description (描述) | 心理學研究所 | zh_TW |
dc.description (描述) | 92752004 | zh_TW |
dc.description (描述) | 95 | zh_TW |
dc.description.abstract (摘要) | 以機率或頻率方式呈現訊息對風險知覺評估的影響,過去的研究有不一致的結果,原因可能來自於判斷事件與受試者的自我相關程度不同,而引發捷思或系統性的訊息處理歷程。實驗一發現當事件的自我相關程度高,受試者會採用系統性的認知處理,而事件的自我相關程度低時,受試者會採用捷思性的認知處理。實驗二詢問受試者認為以機率或頻率方式呈現訊息何者較清楚明確,結果發現有77.5%的受試者認為以頻率方式呈現訊息較以機率方式呈現訊息清楚明確。實驗三操弄「事件自我相關程度」與「機率或頻率方式呈現訊息」,結果發現當事件為高自我相關時,機率或頻率方式呈現訊息在風險知覺的判斷上沒有差異;而當事件為低自我相關時,則頻率方式呈現訊息的「風險知覺」與「事件聯想負向詞數量」均大於機率方式呈現訊息。另外,當事件為低自我相關時,訊息明確度與事件聯想負向詞數量對風險知覺有顯著的預測力。以上的結果支持不同事件自我相關程度會引發捷思或系統性訊息處理,而頻率方式呈現訊息較機率方式呈現訊息清楚明確的原因,與Slovic等人(2000)提出頻率較具體,容易想像的推論符合,但不支持Gigerenzer和Edwards (2003)認為機率的參照類別不清楚的假設。此外,自我相關程度與可得性捷思為影響頻率或機率方式呈現訊息對風險知覺判斷結果不一致的重要變項。 | zh_TW |
dc.description.tableofcontents | 緒論 1文獻探討 2訊息呈現方式對風險知覺的影響 2框架效果 3頻率或機率的呈現方式 4可得性捷思法與風險知覺 7情感對風險知覺的影響 10個別差異與風險知覺 11事件自我相關程度與風險知覺 11自我相關事件的認知處理歷程 13捷思-系統性模式(Heuristic-System Model, HSM) 13推敲可能性模式(Elaboration- Likelihood Model, ELM) 15研究假設 17前測 18實驗一 20實驗二 25實驗三 28綜合討論 36參考文獻 41附件一 47附件二 63附件三 67 | zh_TW |
dc.format.extent | 47966 bytes | - |
dc.format.extent | 75777 bytes | - |
dc.format.extent | 96632 bytes | - |
dc.format.extent | 168382 bytes | - |
dc.format.extent | 605504 bytes | - |
dc.format.extent | 167947 bytes | - |
dc.format.extent | 310042 bytes | - |
dc.format.extent | 389480 bytes | - |
dc.format.extent | 322050 bytes | - |
dc.format.extent | 409176 bytes | - |
dc.format.extent | 282502 bytes | - |
dc.format.extent | 80293 bytes | - |
dc.format.extent | 186484 bytes | - |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | - |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | - |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | - |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | - |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | - |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | - |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | - |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | - |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | - |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | - |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | - |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | - |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | - |
dc.language.iso | en_US | - |
dc.source.uri (資料來源) | http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G0927520041 | en_US |
dc.subject (關鍵詞) | 頻率或機率方式呈現訊息 | zh_TW |
dc.subject (關鍵詞) | 事件自我相關程度 | zh_TW |
dc.subject (關鍵詞) | 推敲可能性模式 | zh_TW |
dc.subject (關鍵詞) | 風險知覺 | zh_TW |
dc.title (題名) | 從事件自我相關程度探討訊息呈現方式對風險知覺之影響 | zh_TW |
dc.type (資料類型) | thesis | en |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic Versus Systematic Information Processing and the Use of Source Versus Message Cues in Persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 752-766. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Chattopadhyay, A. (1998). Heuristic processing can bias systematic processing: Effects of source credibility, argument ambiguity, and task importance on attitude judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 460-473. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Edwards, A., Elwyn, G., Covey, J., Matthews, E., & Pill, R. (2001). Presenting risk information - A review of the effects of "framing`` and other manipulations on patient outcomes. Journal of Health Communication, 6(1), 61-82. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Fetting, J. H., Siminoff, L. A., Piantadosi, S., Abeloff, M. D., Damron, D. J., & Sarsfield, A. M. (1990). Effect of patients` expectations of benefit with standard breast cancer adjuvant chemotherapy on participation in a randomized clinical trial: A clinical vignette study. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 8(9), 1476-1482. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Fischhoff, B. (1996). The real world: What good is it? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 63, 232-248. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Gigerenzer, G. (1996). The psychology of good judgment: Frequency formats and simple algorithms. Medical Decision Making, 16(3), 273-280. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Gigerenzer, G. (2002). Insight. In G. Gigerenzer (Ed.), Calculated risks: How to know when numbers deceive you (pp. 39-51). New York: Simon & Schuster. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Gigerenzer, G., & Edwards, A. (2003). Simple tools for understanding risk: From innumeracy to insight. British Medical Journal, 327, 741-744. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Gigerenzer, G., Hertwig, R., Broek, E. v. d., Fasolo, B., & Katsikopoulos, K. V. (2005). "A 30% chance of rain tomorrow": How does the public understand probabilistic weather forecasts? Risk Analysis, 25(3), 623-629. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Gigerenzer, G., Hoffrage, U., & Kleinbolting, H. (1991). Probabilistic mental models: A Brunswikian theory of confidence. Psychological Review, 98(4), 506-528. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Gorn, G. J. (1982). The effects of music in advertising on choice behavior: A classical conditioning approach. Journal of Marketing, 46(1), 94-101. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Greenwald, A. G. (1968). Cognitive learning, cognitive response to persuasion, and attitude change. In A. G. Greenwald, T. C. Brock & T. M. Ostrom (Eds.), Psychological foundations of attitudes (pp. 147-170). New York: Academic Press. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Griffin, R. J., Dunwoody, S., & Neuwirth, K. (1999). A proposed model of the relationship of risk information seeking and processing to the development of preventative behaviors. Environmental Research, 80, 230-245. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Han, P. K. J., Moser, R. P., & Klein, W. M. P. (2006). Perceived ambiguity about cancer prevention recommendations: Relationship to perceptions of cancer preventability, risk, and worry. Journal of Health Communication, 11, 51-69. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Hellesoy, O., Gronhaug, K., & Kvitastein, O. (1998). Profiling the high hazard perceivers: A exploratory study. Risk Analysis, 18, 253-359. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Johnson, R. J., McCaul, K. D., & Klein, W. M. (2002). Risk involvement and risk perception among adolescents and young adults. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 25(1), 67-82. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect Theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Kan, S., Hongxia, F., Jianming, J., Wendong, L., Zhaoli, S., Jing, G., et al. (2003). The risk perceptioins of SARS and socio-psychological behaviors of urban people in China. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 35(4), 546-554. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Keller, C., Siegrist, M., & Gutscher, H. (2006). The role of the affect and availability heuristics in risk communication. Risk Analysis, 26(3), 631-639. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Kuhn, K. M. (1997). Communicating uncertainty: Framing effects on responses to vague probabilities. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 71(1), 55-83. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Lichtenstein, S., Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B., Layman, M., & Combs, B. (1978). Judged frequency of lethal events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 551-578. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | McGuire, W. J. (1968). Personality and attitude change: An information processing theory. In A. G. Greenwald, T. C. Brock & T. M. Ostrom (Eds.), Psychological foundations of attitudes (pp. 171-196). New York: Academic Press. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Monahan, J., Heilbrun, K., Silver, E., Nabors, E., Bone, J., & Slovic, P. (2002). Communicating violence risk: Frequency formats, vivid outcomes, and forensic settings. International Journal of Forensic Mental Health, 1(2), 121-126. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Myers, J. R., Henderson-King, D. H., & Henderson-King, E. I. (1997). Facing technological risks: The importance of individual differences. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 1-20. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Natter, H. M., & Berry, D. C. (2005). Effects of active information processing on the understanding of risk information. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19(1), 123-135. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | O`Connor, A. (2003, October 7). Finding the fact: Myth about lung cancer can be deadly. Noew York Times. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1992). Behavioral decision research: A constructive processing perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 83-131. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Peters, E., McCaul, K. D., Stefanek, M., & Nelson, W. (2006). A heuristics approach to understanding cancer risk perception: Contributions from judgment and decision-making research. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 31(1), 45-52. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1981). Attitudes and persuasion: classic and contemporary approaches. Dubuque, IA: W. C. Brown. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1984). Source factors and the elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. Advances in Consumer Research, 11, 668-672. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Schumann, D. (1983). Central and peripheral routes to advertising effectiveness: The moderating role of involvement. Journal of Consumer Research, 10(2), 135-146. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Purchase, I. F. H., & Slovic, P. (1999). Quantitative risk assessment breeds fear. Human and Ecological Risk Assessment, 5(3), 445-453. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Rook, K. S. (1987). Effects of case history versus abstract information on health attitudes and behaviors. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 17(6), 533-553. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Ropeik, D. (2002, 20 Oct.). Be afraid of being very afraid. Washingtonpost, p. 1. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Rothman, A. J., Salovey, P., Antone, C., Keough, K., & Martin, C. D. (1993). The influence of message framing on intentions to perform health behaviors. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 29(5), 408-433. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Rothman, A. J., & Schwarz, N. (1998). Constructing perceptions of vulnerability: Personal relevance and the use of experiential information in health judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1053-1064. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Sarfati, D., Howden-Chapman, P., & Woodward, S. C. (1998). Does the frame affect the picture? A study into how attitudes to screening for cancer are affected by the way benefits are expressed. Journal of Medical Screening, 5, 137-140. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Sherif, M., & Hovland, C. I. (1961). Social Judgment: Assimilation, and Contrast Effects in Communication and Attitude Change. New Haven, CT,: Yale University Press. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Slovic, P. (1992). Perceptions of risk: Reflections on the psychometric paradigm. In S. Krimsky & D. Golding (Eds.), Social theories of risk (pp. 117-152). Westport, CT: Praeger. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Slovic, P. (1999). Trust, emotion, sex, politics, and science: Surveying the battlefield. Risk Analysis, 19(4), 689-701. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2004). Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Analysis, 24(2), 311-322. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Slovic, P., Fishchhoff, B., & Lichtenstein, S. (1982a). Facts versus fears: Understanding perceived risk. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 463-489). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Slovic, P., Fishchhoff, B., & Lichtenstein, S. (1982b). Why study risk perception? Risk Analysis, 2, 83-93. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Slovic, P., Monahan, J., & MacGregor, D. G. (2000). Violence risk assessment and risk communication: The effects of using actual cases, providing instruction, and employing probability versus frequency formats. Law and Human Behavior, 24(3), 271-296. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Spielberg, S. (Writer) (1975). Jaws. In R. D. Zanuck & D. Brown (Producer). United States: Universal Pictures. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Szalay, L. B., & Deese, J. (1978). Subjective meaning and culture: An assessment through word associations. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124-1131. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the rationality of choice. Science, 221(453-458). | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Welkenhuysen, M., Evers-Kiebooms, G., & d`Ydewalle, G. (2001). The language of uncertainty in genetic risk communication: framing and verbal versus numerical information. Patient Education and Counseling, 43(2), 179-187. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | Yamagishi, K. (1997). When a 12.86% mortality is more dangerous than 24.14%: Implications for risk communication. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11(6), 495-506. | zh_TW |
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) | | zh_TW |