Publications-Theses
Article View/Open
Publication Export
-
Google ScholarTM
NCCU Library
Citation Infomation
Related Publications in TAIR
題名 台灣華語正常及構音異常幼童之習得現象:滑音產製研究
Phonological Development and Disorder in Taiwan Mandarin:The Status of Glides作者 許馨云
Hsu, Hsin Yun貢獻者 萬依萍
Wan, I Ping
許馨云
Hsu, Hsin Yun關鍵詞 滑音習得
標記理論
位置層級理論
台灣華語
glide development
markedness theory
positional prominence hierarchy
Taiwan Mandarin日期 2016 上傳時間 2-Aug-2016 15:43:49 (UTC+8) 摘要 本篇論文檢視台灣華語為母語之正常幼童以及構音異常幼童之滑音[j],[w], [ɥ]發展與產製表現,採長期觀察之方式,詳細描述正常幼兒滑音產製之出現及穩定年齡、頻率、正確率和發展順序,並且比較此三滑音於各音節位置之產製表現,進而比較兩組幼童之台灣華語滑音產製及音韻歷程之使用行為。本研究以標記理論及位置層級理論來檢驗幼兒滑音之發展與表現。本研究總共觀察了四位幼童,將其分為兩組,第一組為兩位正常幼童,年齡在九個月至二十八個月和十個月至二十九個月,為期十九個月的觀察,另一組為兩位構音異常幼童,年齡在三歲十個月至四歲三個月和四歲三個月至四歲九個月,為期六個月的觀察。每兩週收錄一次長達一小時之語料,並利用錄製之高規格影音檔做譯寫及分析。研究結果顯示,正常幼兒之滑音出現順序和穩定順序皆符合標記理論之預測,無標音早於有標音,其滑音產製之穩定度與音節位置相關,音節首位之滑音表現較音節中與音節末之滑音表現來的穩定,這與位置層級理論之推測相符合。構音異常幼童組中發現了有別於正常幼童之特殊取代模式,由較晚習得語音取代較早習得語音,其所產製的語音中會違反華語的音法限制。音韻歷程之表現在兩組幼童中皆偵測到刪除、換位、取代三種模式,其中皆以刪除為主要策略。
The purpose of the present study is to report the developmental process of three Mandarin glides [w, j, ɥ] in terms of three word positions by examining the age of emergence and stabilization, the order of stabilization, and accuracy rate of thechildren’s production, and further compare the normative data with phonological disorder data in order to explore the possible phonological processes. This study alsoaccounts for the developmental process of glides on the basis of markedness theory and positional prominence hierarchy.A longitudinal study was carried out for the investigation of two normally-developing children, aged between 0;9-2;4 and 0;10-2;4, and two phonologically-disordered children, one of whom is between 4;3 and 4;9, and the other between 3;10 and 4;3. The data were collected at two-week intervals.The results showed that the order of glide emergence and stabilization of the normally-developing group is in accordance with the markedness theory. The unmarked [j], [w] precede marked [ɥ]. Moreover, the stabilization order of the three glides in terms of the three syllable positions was found to reflect the interaction between markedness constraint and positional prominence hierarchy. The unmarked glide in the initial position is the first to stabilize and the marked glide in the non-initial position is the last to stabilize. On the other hand, the children in the phonologically-disordered group were found to consistently replace the presumably earlier-developing glide with a presumably later-developing one, which differs from the process used in the normally-developing group. Furthermore, this group of children produced the combination that violates the phonotactic constraints of Taiwan Mandarin. In addition, there are three phonological processes, including deletion, metathesis, and substitution detected in children’s data. The most commonly used process is deletion in both groups of children.參考文獻 Bao, Z. (1990). Fanqie languages and reduplication. Linguistic Inquiry, 317-350.Bao, Z. (1996). The syllable in Chinese. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 24(2), 312-353.Bao, Z. (2002). The asymmetry of the medial glides in Middle Chinese. Chinese Phonology, 11, 7-27.Bortolini, U., & Leonard, L. (1991). The speech of phonologically-disordered children acquiring Italian. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 5, 1-12.Bradford, B., & Dodd, B. (1994). The motor planning abilities of phonologically-disordered children. European Journal of Disorders of Communication, 29, 349–369.Chao, Y.-R. (1951). The Cantian idiolect: an analysis of the Chinese spoken by a twenty-eight-month-old child. In C. A. Ferguson & D. I. Slobin (eds), Studies of child language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, INC.Chao, Y.-R. (1968). A grammar of spoken Chinese. Berkeley: University of California Press.119Cheng, C.-C. (1973). A synchronic phonology of Mandarin Chinese. Mouton: The Hague.Crothers, J. (1978). Typology and universals of vowel systems in phonology. Universals of human language, 2, 95-152.Demuth, K., & Fee, J. (1995). Minimal prosodic words in early phonological development. Ms., Brown University and Dalhousie University.Demuth, K., Culbertson, J., & Alter, J. (2006). Word-minimality, epenthesis and coda licensing in the early acquisition of English. Language and Speech, 49(2), 137-173.Duanmu. S. 1990. A Formal Study of Syllable, Tone, Stress, and Domain in Chinese Languages. Cambridge: MIT dissertation.Duanmu, S. (2002). The phonology of Standard Chinese. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Dodd, B., Holm, A., Zhu, H., & Crosbie, S. (2003). Phonological development: A normative study of British English-speaking children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 17, 617–643.Ferguson, C. A., & Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Language, 419-439.Gick, B. (2003). Articulatory correlates of ambisyllabicity in English glides and liquids. Phonetic Interpretation: Papers in Laboratory Phonology VI, 222-236.Gleason, J. B., & Ratner, N. B. (2009). The development of language, 7th ed. Boston, MA: Pearson.Gnanadesikan, A. E. (1996). Child phonology in optimality theory: Ranking markedness and faithfulness constraints. In Proceedings of the 20th Annual Boston University Conference on language development, 1, 237-248.Goldstein, B. A. (2005). Substitution patterns in the phonology of Spanish-speaking children. Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 3(3), 153-168.Greenberg, J. (1966). Language universals; with special reference to feature hierarchies. Janua linguarum, series minor, 59. The Hague: Mouton.Grunwell, P. (1981). The nature of phonological disability in children. Academic Press.Grunwell, P. (1982). Clinical phonology. Rockville, MD: Aspen Publishers.Grunwell, P. (1987). Clinical phonology (2nd ed.). London: Croom Helm.Hawkins, J. A., & Cutler, A. (1988). Psycholinguistic factors in morphological asymmetry. Explaining language universals, 280-317.Hedrick, D. L., Prather, E. M., and Tobin, A. R., (1975). Sequenced Inventory of Communication Development. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press.Ingram, D. (1988). The acquisition of word-initial [v]. Language and Speech, 31, 77–85.Jakobson, R. (1941). Kindersprache, Aphasie, und allgemeine Lautgesetze. Uppsala, Almqvist & Wiksell: Child Language, Aphasia and Phonological Universals. The Hague: Mouton.Jakobson, R. (1968). Child language, aphasia, and phonological universals. The Hauge: Mouton.Jeng, H-H. (1979). The acquisition of Chinese phonology in relation to Jakobson’s laws of irreversible solidarity. Proceedings of the 9th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. University of Copenhagen, 2, 155-161.Jiménez, J., & Lloret, M. R. (2013). Vocalic adjustments under positional markedness in Catalan and other Romance languages. Information Structure and Agreement, 197, 319-336.Kirk, C., & Demuth, K. (2006). Accounting for variability in 2-year-olds` production of coda consonants. Language Learning and Development, 2, 97–118.Krakow, R. A. (1989). The articulatory organization of syllables: A kinematic analysis of labial and velar gestures. University Microfilms.Kochetov, A. (2006). Syllable position effects and gestural organization: articulatory evidence from Russian. In Goldstein, L., Whalen, D. & Best, C. (eds.), Papers in Laboratory Phonology VIII: Varieties of Phonological Competence. Berlin: Mouton deGruyter.Ladefoged, P., & Johnson, K. (2014). A Course in Phonetics (7th ed.). Stamford, CT: Cengage.Leonard, L. (1985). Unusual and subtle phonological behavior in the speech of phonologically-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 50, 4-13. (1985 Editor`s Award: Article of Highest Merit)Li, Paul J.-K. (1977). Child language acquisition of Mandarin phonology. In R.Cheng, Y. C. Li & Ting-chi Tang (eds), Proceedings of the Symposium on Chinese Linguistics: Linguistic Institute of the Linguistic Society of America. Taipei: Student Books.Lin, Y.-H. (1990). Prenuclear glides in Chinese. In Mid-America Linguistics Conference.Lin, Y.-H. (1989). Autosegmental treatment of segmental processes in Chinese phonology. Ph D. Dissertation, the University of Texas, Austin.Lin, Y-H. (2007). The Sounds of Chinese. New York: Cambridge University Press.Lindblom, B., & Maddieson, I. (1988). Phonetic universals in consonant systems. Language, speech, and mind, 62-78.Lorentz, J. (1974). A deviant phonological system of English. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 8, 55-64.Menn, L. I. S. E. (1976). Pattern, control, and contrast in beginning speech: A case study in the acquisition of word form and function. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois.Nooteboom, S. G. (1981). Lexical retrieval from fragments of spoken words: Beginnings vs. endings. Journal of Phonetics, 9(4), 407-424.Oller, D.K. (1980). The emergence of the sounds of speech in infancy. In G. Yeni-Komshian, J. Kavanagh, & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Child phonology, Vol. 1: Production (pp. 93–112). New York: Academic Press.Olmsted, D. (1971). Out of the mouths of babes. The Hague: Mouton.O`Grady, W. D., Archibald, M., & Katamba, F. (2011). Contemporary linguistics: An introduction, 2nd ed. London: Longman.Prather, E. M., Hedrick, D. L., & Kern, C. A. (1975). Articulation development in children aged two to four years. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders,40(2), 179-191.Poole, I. (1934). Genetic development of articulation of consonant sounds in speech. The Elementary English Review, 159-161.Sander, E. K. (1972). When are speech sounds learned?. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 37(1), 55-63.Shriberg, L. D., & Kwiatkowski, J. (1982). Phonological disorders I: A diagnostic classification system. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 47, 226–241.Smit, A. B., Hand, L., Freilinger, J. J., Bernthal, J. E., & Bird, A. (1990). The Iowa Articulation Norms Project and its Nebraska replication. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 779–798.Smith, N. V. (1973). The acquisition of phonology: A case study. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.So, L., & Dodd, B. (1995). The acquisition of phonology by Cantonese-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 22(03), 473-495. doi: doi:10.1017/S0305000900009922Sosa, V. A., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (2012). Lexical and Phonological Effects in Early Word Production. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 596-608.Stoel-Gammon, C. (1985). Phonetic inventories: 15–22 months. A longitudinal study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 506–512.Stites, J., Demuth, K., & Kirk, C. (2004). Markedness versus frequency effects in coda acquisition. In Alejna Brugos, Linnea Micciulla, & Christine E. Smith (Eds.), Proceedings of the 28th annual Boston University conference on language development (pp. 565–576). Sommerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Stoel-Gammon, C., & Dunn, C. (1985). Normal and disordered phonology in children. Austin, Texas: Pro-Ed.Su, A-T. (1985). The acquisition of Mandarin phonology by Taiwanese children. MA. thesis, Fu Jen Catholic University.Theodore, R. M., Demuth, K., & Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (2012). Segmental and positional effects on children`s coda production: Comparing evidence from perceptual judgments and acoustic analysis. Clinical linguistics & phonetics, 26(9), 755-773.Trubetzkoy, N. S. (1939). Grundzüge der Phonologie. Prague: Cercle Linguistique dePrague. Translated 1969 as Principles of Phonology by Christine A. M. Baltaxe. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.Vihman, M. M., Macken, M. A., Miller, R., Simmons, H., & Miller, J.. (1985). From Babbling to Speech: A Re-Assessment of the Continuity Issue. Language, 61(2), 397–445.Vihman, M. M., & Miller, R. (1986). Words and babble at the threshold of lexical acquisition. In M.D. Smith& J.L. Locke (Eds.), The emergent lexicon: The child`s development of a linguistic vocabulary. New York: Academic Press.Wan, I. P. (1997). The status of prenuclear glides in Mandarin Chinese: Evidence from speech errors. Chicago Linguistics Society, 33, 417-428.Wan, I. P. (1999). Mandarin Phonology: Evidence from Speech Errors. PhD Dissertation, University of New York at Buffalo.Wan, I. P. (2003). Alignments of prenuclear glides in Mandarin. Taipei: Crane Publishing.Wan, I. P. & J. Jaeger (2003) “The phonological representation of Taiwan Mandarin vowels: A psycholinguistic study,” Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 12, 205-257.Wang, H. S., & Chang, C-L. (2001). On the Status of the Prenucleus Glide in Mandarin Chinese. Language and Linguistic, 2(2), 243-260.Weiner, F. F. (1981). Systematic sound preference as a characteristic of phonological disability. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 46(3), 281-286.Wellman, B. L., Case, I. M., Mengert, I. G., & Bradbury, D. E. (1931). Speech sounds of young children. University of Iowa Studies: Child Welfare.Templin, M. (1957). Certain language skills in children: Their development and interrelationships. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Wu, Y. (1994). Mandarin segmental phonology. Ph D. Dissertation, University of Toronto.Zhu, H. (2000). Phonological development and disorder of Putonghua (ModernStandard Chinese)-speaking children. (PhD thesis), University of Newcastle upon Tyne, UK.Zhu, H. (2002). Phonological Development in Specific Contexts: Studies of Chinese-Speaking Children. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.吳咸蘭 (1999)。構音與音韻障礙的治療,於曾進興主編 語言病理學基礎, 第三卷 心理出版社。林寶貴、林美秀 (1993)。學齡前兒童語言障礙評量指導手冊。台北市:國立台灣師範大學特殊教育研究所編印。許洪坤 (1987)。中國兒童學習國語及語法發展階段研究。國家科學委員會研究報告。 描述 碩士
國立政治大學
語言學研究所
102555011資料來源 http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G1025550112 資料類型 thesis dc.contributor.advisor 萬依萍 zh_TW dc.contributor.advisor Wan, I Ping en_US dc.contributor.author (Authors) 許馨云 zh_TW dc.contributor.author (Authors) Hsu, Hsin Yun en_US dc.creator (作者) 許馨云 zh_TW dc.creator (作者) Hsu, Hsin Yun en_US dc.date (日期) 2016 en_US dc.date.accessioned 2-Aug-2016 15:43:49 (UTC+8) - dc.date.available 2-Aug-2016 15:43:49 (UTC+8) - dc.date.issued (上傳時間) 2-Aug-2016 15:43:49 (UTC+8) - dc.identifier (Other Identifiers) G1025550112 en_US dc.identifier.uri (URI) http://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/99514 - dc.description (描述) 碩士 zh_TW dc.description (描述) 國立政治大學 zh_TW dc.description (描述) 語言學研究所 zh_TW dc.description (描述) 102555011 zh_TW dc.description.abstract (摘要) 本篇論文檢視台灣華語為母語之正常幼童以及構音異常幼童之滑音[j],[w], [ɥ]發展與產製表現,採長期觀察之方式,詳細描述正常幼兒滑音產製之出現及穩定年齡、頻率、正確率和發展順序,並且比較此三滑音於各音節位置之產製表現,進而比較兩組幼童之台灣華語滑音產製及音韻歷程之使用行為。本研究以標記理論及位置層級理論來檢驗幼兒滑音之發展與表現。本研究總共觀察了四位幼童,將其分為兩組,第一組為兩位正常幼童,年齡在九個月至二十八個月和十個月至二十九個月,為期十九個月的觀察,另一組為兩位構音異常幼童,年齡在三歲十個月至四歲三個月和四歲三個月至四歲九個月,為期六個月的觀察。每兩週收錄一次長達一小時之語料,並利用錄製之高規格影音檔做譯寫及分析。研究結果顯示,正常幼兒之滑音出現順序和穩定順序皆符合標記理論之預測,無標音早於有標音,其滑音產製之穩定度與音節位置相關,音節首位之滑音表現較音節中與音節末之滑音表現來的穩定,這與位置層級理論之推測相符合。構音異常幼童組中發現了有別於正常幼童之特殊取代模式,由較晚習得語音取代較早習得語音,其所產製的語音中會違反華語的音法限制。音韻歷程之表現在兩組幼童中皆偵測到刪除、換位、取代三種模式,其中皆以刪除為主要策略。 zh_TW dc.description.abstract (摘要) The purpose of the present study is to report the developmental process of three Mandarin glides [w, j, ɥ] in terms of three word positions by examining the age of emergence and stabilization, the order of stabilization, and accuracy rate of thechildren’s production, and further compare the normative data with phonological disorder data in order to explore the possible phonological processes. This study alsoaccounts for the developmental process of glides on the basis of markedness theory and positional prominence hierarchy.A longitudinal study was carried out for the investigation of two normally-developing children, aged between 0;9-2;4 and 0;10-2;4, and two phonologically-disordered children, one of whom is between 4;3 and 4;9, and the other between 3;10 and 4;3. The data were collected at two-week intervals.The results showed that the order of glide emergence and stabilization of the normally-developing group is in accordance with the markedness theory. The unmarked [j], [w] precede marked [ɥ]. Moreover, the stabilization order of the three glides in terms of the three syllable positions was found to reflect the interaction between markedness constraint and positional prominence hierarchy. The unmarked glide in the initial position is the first to stabilize and the marked glide in the non-initial position is the last to stabilize. On the other hand, the children in the phonologically-disordered group were found to consistently replace the presumably earlier-developing glide with a presumably later-developing one, which differs from the process used in the normally-developing group. Furthermore, this group of children produced the combination that violates the phonotactic constraints of Taiwan Mandarin. In addition, there are three phonological processes, including deletion, metathesis, and substitution detected in children’s data. The most commonly used process is deletion in both groups of children. en_US dc.description.tableofcontents Chapter 1 Introduction 11.1 Segmental acquisition of glides 21.2 The phonetic and phonemic approaches 51.3 Phonologically-disordered children 71.4 Research questions 91.5 The framework of this study 11Chapter 2 Literature review 132.1 Phonological development of glides 132.1.1 Glides acquisition of normally and phonologically-disordered children 142.1.2 Glide development in Mandarin children 242.2 An introduction to Mandarin 292.2.1 Mandarin consonants and vowels 292.2.2 Phonological accounts on Mandarin glides 332.2.3 Phonotactic of glide sequences 362.3 Phonological acquisition theories 412.3.1 Markedness theory 412.3.2 Positional effect and positional prominence hierarchy 44Chapter 3 Methodology 483.1 Data collection 493.1.1 Participants 503.1.2 Data collecting procedures 523.1.3 Recording apparatus 533.2 Data analysis 543.2.1 Transcription & Coding 553.2.2 Criteria for data selection 573.2.3 Glide emergence and stabilization assessment 58Chapter 4 Findings and analysis 614.1 Emergence of glides in normally-developing group 614.2 Stabilization of glides in normally-developing group 644.2.1 Data and PCC/PCE 644.2.2 Order of stabilization of glides 854.3 Glide performance of the phonologically-disordered group 924.4 Phonological processes and patterns 954.4.1 Tokens of Phonological Processes 954.4.2 Percentages of phonological processes 100Chapter 5 Discussion 1035.1 Summary of the study 1035.2 Interaction of markedness and positional prominence hierarchy 1045.2.1 Markedness and positional prominence hierarchy in glide emergence 1045.2.2 Markedness and positional prominence hierarchy in glide stabilization 1065.3 Comparison of glide performance in the two groups 1095.3.1 Glide performance in the two groups 1105.3.2 phonological processes in the two groups 1135.4 Concluding remarks 114References 118 zh_TW dc.format.extent 2814116 bytes - dc.format.mimetype application/pdf - dc.source.uri (資料來源) http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G1025550112 en_US dc.subject (關鍵詞) 滑音習得 zh_TW dc.subject (關鍵詞) 標記理論 zh_TW dc.subject (關鍵詞) 位置層級理論 zh_TW dc.subject (關鍵詞) 台灣華語 zh_TW dc.subject (關鍵詞) glide development en_US dc.subject (關鍵詞) markedness theory en_US dc.subject (關鍵詞) positional prominence hierarchy en_US dc.subject (關鍵詞) Taiwan Mandarin en_US dc.title (題名) 台灣華語正常及構音異常幼童之習得現象:滑音產製研究 zh_TW dc.title (題名) Phonological Development and Disorder in Taiwan Mandarin:The Status of Glides en_US dc.type (資料類型) thesis en_US dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Bao, Z. (1990). Fanqie languages and reduplication. Linguistic Inquiry, 317-350.Bao, Z. (1996). The syllable in Chinese. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 24(2), 312-353.Bao, Z. (2002). The asymmetry of the medial glides in Middle Chinese. Chinese Phonology, 11, 7-27.Bortolini, U., & Leonard, L. (1991). The speech of phonologically-disordered children acquiring Italian. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 5, 1-12.Bradford, B., & Dodd, B. (1994). The motor planning abilities of phonologically-disordered children. European Journal of Disorders of Communication, 29, 349–369.Chao, Y.-R. (1951). The Cantian idiolect: an analysis of the Chinese spoken by a twenty-eight-month-old child. In C. A. Ferguson & D. I. Slobin (eds), Studies of child language development. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, INC.Chao, Y.-R. (1968). A grammar of spoken Chinese. Berkeley: University of California Press.119Cheng, C.-C. (1973). A synchronic phonology of Mandarin Chinese. Mouton: The Hague.Crothers, J. (1978). Typology and universals of vowel systems in phonology. Universals of human language, 2, 95-152.Demuth, K., & Fee, J. (1995). Minimal prosodic words in early phonological development. Ms., Brown University and Dalhousie University.Demuth, K., Culbertson, J., & Alter, J. (2006). Word-minimality, epenthesis and coda licensing in the early acquisition of English. Language and Speech, 49(2), 137-173.Duanmu. S. 1990. A Formal Study of Syllable, Tone, Stress, and Domain in Chinese Languages. Cambridge: MIT dissertation.Duanmu, S. (2002). The phonology of Standard Chinese. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Dodd, B., Holm, A., Zhu, H., & Crosbie, S. (2003). Phonological development: A normative study of British English-speaking children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 17, 617–643.Ferguson, C. A., & Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Language, 419-439.Gick, B. (2003). Articulatory correlates of ambisyllabicity in English glides and liquids. Phonetic Interpretation: Papers in Laboratory Phonology VI, 222-236.Gleason, J. B., & Ratner, N. B. (2009). The development of language, 7th ed. Boston, MA: Pearson.Gnanadesikan, A. E. (1996). Child phonology in optimality theory: Ranking markedness and faithfulness constraints. In Proceedings of the 20th Annual Boston University Conference on language development, 1, 237-248.Goldstein, B. A. (2005). Substitution patterns in the phonology of Spanish-speaking children. Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 3(3), 153-168.Greenberg, J. (1966). Language universals; with special reference to feature hierarchies. Janua linguarum, series minor, 59. The Hague: Mouton.Grunwell, P. (1981). The nature of phonological disability in children. Academic Press.Grunwell, P. (1982). Clinical phonology. Rockville, MD: Aspen Publishers.Grunwell, P. (1987). Clinical phonology (2nd ed.). London: Croom Helm.Hawkins, J. A., & Cutler, A. (1988). Psycholinguistic factors in morphological asymmetry. Explaining language universals, 280-317.Hedrick, D. L., Prather, E. M., and Tobin, A. R., (1975). Sequenced Inventory of Communication Development. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press.Ingram, D. (1988). The acquisition of word-initial [v]. Language and Speech, 31, 77–85.Jakobson, R. (1941). Kindersprache, Aphasie, und allgemeine Lautgesetze. Uppsala, Almqvist & Wiksell: Child Language, Aphasia and Phonological Universals. The Hague: Mouton.Jakobson, R. (1968). Child language, aphasia, and phonological universals. The Hauge: Mouton.Jeng, H-H. (1979). The acquisition of Chinese phonology in relation to Jakobson’s laws of irreversible solidarity. Proceedings of the 9th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. University of Copenhagen, 2, 155-161.Jiménez, J., & Lloret, M. R. (2013). Vocalic adjustments under positional markedness in Catalan and other Romance languages. Information Structure and Agreement, 197, 319-336.Kirk, C., & Demuth, K. (2006). Accounting for variability in 2-year-olds` production of coda consonants. Language Learning and Development, 2, 97–118.Krakow, R. A. (1989). The articulatory organization of syllables: A kinematic analysis of labial and velar gestures. University Microfilms.Kochetov, A. (2006). Syllable position effects and gestural organization: articulatory evidence from Russian. In Goldstein, L., Whalen, D. & Best, C. (eds.), Papers in Laboratory Phonology VIII: Varieties of Phonological Competence. Berlin: Mouton deGruyter.Ladefoged, P., & Johnson, K. (2014). A Course in Phonetics (7th ed.). Stamford, CT: Cengage.Leonard, L. (1985). Unusual and subtle phonological behavior in the speech of phonologically-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 50, 4-13. (1985 Editor`s Award: Article of Highest Merit)Li, Paul J.-K. (1977). Child language acquisition of Mandarin phonology. In R.Cheng, Y. C. Li & Ting-chi Tang (eds), Proceedings of the Symposium on Chinese Linguistics: Linguistic Institute of the Linguistic Society of America. Taipei: Student Books.Lin, Y.-H. (1990). Prenuclear glides in Chinese. In Mid-America Linguistics Conference.Lin, Y.-H. (1989). Autosegmental treatment of segmental processes in Chinese phonology. Ph D. Dissertation, the University of Texas, Austin.Lin, Y-H. (2007). The Sounds of Chinese. New York: Cambridge University Press.Lindblom, B., & Maddieson, I. (1988). Phonetic universals in consonant systems. Language, speech, and mind, 62-78.Lorentz, J. (1974). A deviant phonological system of English. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 8, 55-64.Menn, L. I. S. E. (1976). Pattern, control, and contrast in beginning speech: A case study in the acquisition of word form and function. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois.Nooteboom, S. G. (1981). Lexical retrieval from fragments of spoken words: Beginnings vs. endings. Journal of Phonetics, 9(4), 407-424.Oller, D.K. (1980). The emergence of the sounds of speech in infancy. In G. Yeni-Komshian, J. Kavanagh, & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Child phonology, Vol. 1: Production (pp. 93–112). New York: Academic Press.Olmsted, D. (1971). Out of the mouths of babes. The Hague: Mouton.O`Grady, W. D., Archibald, M., & Katamba, F. (2011). Contemporary linguistics: An introduction, 2nd ed. London: Longman.Prather, E. M., Hedrick, D. L., & Kern, C. A. (1975). Articulation development in children aged two to four years. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders,40(2), 179-191.Poole, I. (1934). Genetic development of articulation of consonant sounds in speech. The Elementary English Review, 159-161.Sander, E. K. (1972). When are speech sounds learned?. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 37(1), 55-63.Shriberg, L. D., & Kwiatkowski, J. (1982). Phonological disorders I: A diagnostic classification system. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 47, 226–241.Smit, A. B., Hand, L., Freilinger, J. J., Bernthal, J. E., & Bird, A. (1990). The Iowa Articulation Norms Project and its Nebraska replication. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 779–798.Smith, N. V. (1973). The acquisition of phonology: A case study. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.So, L., & Dodd, B. (1995). The acquisition of phonology by Cantonese-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 22(03), 473-495. doi: doi:10.1017/S0305000900009922Sosa, V. A., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (2012). Lexical and Phonological Effects in Early Word Production. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 596-608.Stoel-Gammon, C. (1985). Phonetic inventories: 15–22 months. A longitudinal study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 506–512.Stites, J., Demuth, K., & Kirk, C. (2004). Markedness versus frequency effects in coda acquisition. In Alejna Brugos, Linnea Micciulla, & Christine E. Smith (Eds.), Proceedings of the 28th annual Boston University conference on language development (pp. 565–576). Sommerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Stoel-Gammon, C., & Dunn, C. (1985). Normal and disordered phonology in children. Austin, Texas: Pro-Ed.Su, A-T. (1985). The acquisition of Mandarin phonology by Taiwanese children. MA. thesis, Fu Jen Catholic University.Theodore, R. M., Demuth, K., & Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (2012). Segmental and positional effects on children`s coda production: Comparing evidence from perceptual judgments and acoustic analysis. Clinical linguistics & phonetics, 26(9), 755-773.Trubetzkoy, N. S. (1939). Grundzüge der Phonologie. Prague: Cercle Linguistique dePrague. Translated 1969 as Principles of Phonology by Christine A. M. Baltaxe. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.Vihman, M. M., Macken, M. A., Miller, R., Simmons, H., & Miller, J.. (1985). From Babbling to Speech: A Re-Assessment of the Continuity Issue. Language, 61(2), 397–445.Vihman, M. M., & Miller, R. (1986). Words and babble at the threshold of lexical acquisition. In M.D. Smith& J.L. Locke (Eds.), The emergent lexicon: The child`s development of a linguistic vocabulary. New York: Academic Press.Wan, I. P. (1997). The status of prenuclear glides in Mandarin Chinese: Evidence from speech errors. Chicago Linguistics Society, 33, 417-428.Wan, I. P. (1999). Mandarin Phonology: Evidence from Speech Errors. PhD Dissertation, University of New York at Buffalo.Wan, I. P. (2003). Alignments of prenuclear glides in Mandarin. Taipei: Crane Publishing.Wan, I. P. & J. Jaeger (2003) “The phonological representation of Taiwan Mandarin vowels: A psycholinguistic study,” Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 12, 205-257.Wang, H. S., & Chang, C-L. (2001). On the Status of the Prenucleus Glide in Mandarin Chinese. Language and Linguistic, 2(2), 243-260.Weiner, F. F. (1981). Systematic sound preference as a characteristic of phonological disability. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 46(3), 281-286.Wellman, B. L., Case, I. M., Mengert, I. G., & Bradbury, D. E. (1931). Speech sounds of young children. University of Iowa Studies: Child Welfare.Templin, M. (1957). Certain language skills in children: Their development and interrelationships. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Wu, Y. (1994). Mandarin segmental phonology. Ph D. Dissertation, University of Toronto.Zhu, H. (2000). Phonological development and disorder of Putonghua (ModernStandard Chinese)-speaking children. (PhD thesis), University of Newcastle upon Tyne, UK.Zhu, H. (2002). Phonological Development in Specific Contexts: Studies of Chinese-Speaking Children. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.吳咸蘭 (1999)。構音與音韻障礙的治療,於曾進興主編 語言病理學基礎, 第三卷 心理出版社。林寶貴、林美秀 (1993)。學齡前兒童語言障礙評量指導手冊。台北市:國立台灣師範大學特殊教育研究所編印。許洪坤 (1987)。中國兒童學習國語及語法發展階段研究。國家科學委員會研究報告。 zh_TW