學術產出-Theses

Article View/Open

Publication Export

Google ScholarTM

政大圖書館

Citation Infomation

  • No doi shows Citation Infomation
題名 中文言語產製中語意及語音處理的電生理訊號
Electrophysiological signatures of semantic and phonological processing in mandarin Chinese speech production
作者 張惠娟
Chang, Hui Chuan
貢獻者 郭文瑞<br>薩文蕙
Kuo, Wen Jui<br>Sah, Wen Hui
張惠娟
Chang, Hui Chuan
關鍵詞 事件相關電位
言語產製
習得年齡
側化準備電位
Eevent-related potential
Speech production
Age-of-Acquisition
Lateral readiness potential
日期 2008
上傳時間 14-Sep-2009 12:59:45 (UTC+8)
摘要 習得年齡對語言處理有相當影響,但其成因及作用階段至今仍無定案,根據以知的行為研究成果及假說,結合事件相關電位技術,及雙重選擇按鍵與否典範,我們將嘗試找出習得年齡在言語產出中的作用階段。假定不欲按鍵則無側化動作準備電位,且側化準備電位起始時間取決於,何時取得用以決定反應手的訊息,我們請受試者根據圖片刺激的兩種語言屬性做出正確反應。
      在實驗一中,語意屬性(人造或非人造)決定反應手,圖片名稱第一音節的語音屬性(一、三聲或二、四聲)決定按鍵與否。若習得年齡影響語意處理,我們預期在按鍵情形下,習得年齡早晚兩組的側化準備電位起始時間將有所不同;而不按鍵情形下的側化準備電位持續時間,將反映習得年齡對語音處理的影響。實驗二中,圖片經更嚴格挑選,以控制所有變項,且語言屬性與反應類別間的兩種對應都被測試。
      實驗一和實驗二都發現不按鍵時,若反應手非由語意屬性決定,則側化準備電位不會出現。這個結果符合文獻記載,並暗示語意處理早於語音處理。實驗一中,早習得圖片的唸名反應時間、圖片分類反應時間、及側化準備電位起始時間都較短,支持習得年齡越早,語意提取速度越快。但實驗二中,習得年齡對側化準備電位起始時間沒有顯著影響,儘管前測顯示早習得圖片的唸名反應時間較短,其分類反應時間卻較長,。實驗三進一步肯定,習得年齡對唸名及圖片分類作業有相反效果,以及習得年齡對圖片分類作業的效果不因重複練習而喪失。
      根據完整語音理論 (Brown & Watson, 1987),我們認為當作業要求完整的語音訊息,則習得年齡越早,提取速度越快,唸名作業如是;當作業要求片段的語音訊息,則習得年齡越早,提取速度越慢,額外時間用於解構完整語音訊息,我們的圖片判斷作業如是。實驗二中因少數圖片重複出現,習得年齡對語意處理的正面作用減少,習得年齡對片段語音處理的負面作用卻維持,使得習得年齡組別間,側化準備電位沒有顯著差異,且早習得組圖片分類反應時間反而較長。
      總而言之,本研究支持概念/詞目競爭理論的說法,亦即習得年齡越早,語意提取速度越快。我們的資料也間接反映,習得年齡越早,語音提取速度越慢,但此推論仍需未來研究進一步支持。
Age-of-acquisition (AoA) is one of the most robust variables for language processing. But its cause and locus during the processing are still under debate. Based on previous behavioral studies and established hypotheses, this study aims to find electrophysiological evidence for the locus of AoA effect during speech production. The continuous measure of speech production is approached by event-related potential (ERP) in conjunction with a manual, dual choice Go/Nogo paradigm. In this paradigm, participants need to choose the correct response according to two linguistic attributes of the target picture. The presumption of this paradigm is that the lateralized readiness potential (LRP), a motor preparation related component, would be absent once the Nogo decision has been made, and its onset marks the time point at which critical information for hand selection is available.
      In Experiment 1, which hand to respond depends on the target picture’s semantic attribute (artifact or non-artifact), to respond or not relies on its phonological attribute (the tone value of its first syllable). It is expected that if AoA influences lexical/semantic processing, LRP onset latency would be different under early and late AoA condition. AoA effect on phonological stage would be reflected on the duration of Nogo LRP. In Experiment 2 the reverse mapping between linguistic attribute and response types was also tested and variables other than AoA were more strictly controlled.
      Both experiments replicate previous finding in that Nogo LRP only occurs when response hand is contingent on semantic information, which indicates that semantic stage precedes phonological stage. In Experiment 1, naming latency, picture classification RT (reaction time), and LRP onset latency were shorter for the early AoA group, indicating accelerated lexical/semantic retrieval. However, in Experiment 2, AoA effect on LRP onset latency was not found. And for items with early AoA, picture classification RT is significantly longer, despite that their naming latency is shorter in the pretest. Experiment 3 further validates the coexistence of the positive AoA effect on naming and negative AoA effect on tone classification and the resistance of the negative AoA effect to repetition.
      Following the phonological completeness hypothesis (Brown & Watson, 1987), we suggest that when the unitary phonological form is demanded, as in the naming task, the early learned words has advantage. When a fraction of the unitary form is demanded, as in the tone classification task, extra time is needed to decompose the retrieved unitary form. The positive AoA effect on semantic stage is larger in Experiment 1 due to material selection and number of repetition. Thus in Experiment 1, the positive AoA effect on semantic stage remains, but in Experiment 2, it is overpowered by the negative AoA effect on fractional phonological extraction, resulting in the absence of AoA effect on LRP pattern and longer picture classification RT under early AoA condition.
      In sum, results of this study indicate that semantic feature can be retrieved faster under early AoA condition, which is in line with concept/lemma competition hypothesis. Our results also imply slower tone retrieval under early AoA condition, which need to be further testified by future studies.
參考文獻 Abdel Rahman, R., & Sommer, W. (2003). Does phonological encoding in speech production always follow the retrieval of semantic knowledge?: Electrophysiological evidence for parallel processing. Cognitive Brain Research, 16(3), 372-382.
Abdel Rahman, R., Van Turennout, M., & Levelt, W. J. M. (2003). Phonological Encoding Is Not Contingent on Semantic Feature Retrieval: An Electrophysiological Study on Object Naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(5), 850-860.
Barry, C., Morrison, C. M., & Ellis, A. W. (1997). Naming the Snodgrass and Vanderwart Pictures: Effects of Age of Acquisition, Frequency, and Name Agreement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 50(3), 560-585.
Belke, E., Brysbaert, M., Meyer, A. S., & Ghyselinck, M. (2005). Age of acquisition effects in picture naming: evidence for a lexical-semantic competition hypothesis. Cognition, 96(2), B45-B54.
Bonin, P., Chalard, M., Meot, A., & Barry, C. (2006). Are age-of-acquisition effects on object naming due simply to difference in object recognition? comments on Levelt (2002) Memory & cognition, 34(5), 1172-1182.
Bonin, P., Chalard, M., Meot, A., & Fayol, M. (2002). The determinants of spoken and written picture naming latencies. British Journal of Psychology, 93(1), 89.
Bonin, P., Fayol, M., & Chalard, M. (2001). Age of acquisition and word frequency in written picture naming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 54(2), 469-489.
Brown, G. D., & Watson, F. L. (1987). First in, first out: word learning age and spoken word frequency as predictors of word familiarity and word naming latency. Memory & Cognition, 15(3), 208-216.
Brysbaert, M., & Ghyselinck, M. (2006). The effect of age of acquisition: Partly frequency related, partly frequency independent. Visual Cognition, 13(7/8), 992-1011.
Brysbaert, M., Van Wijnendaele, I., & De Deyne, S. (2000). Age-of-acquisition effects in semantic processing tasks. Acta Psychologica, 104(2), 215-226.
Carroll, J. B., & White, M. N. (1973). Word frequency and age-of-acquisition as determiners of picture-naming latency. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 85–95.
Catling, J. C., & Johnston, R. A. (2006). The effects of age of acquisition on an object classification task. Visual Cognition, 13(7/8), 968-980.
Chalard, M., & Bonin, P. (2006). Age-of-acquisition effects in picture naming: Are they structural and/or semantic in nature? Visual Cognition, 13(7/8), 864-883.
Chen, J.-Y., Chen, T.-M., & Dell, G. S. (2002). Word-form encoding in Mandarin Chinese as assessed by the implicit priming task. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(4), 751-781.
Chen, J.-Y., Wei-Chun, L., & Ludovic, F. (2003). Masked priming of the syllable in Chinese speech production. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 45(1), 107-120.
Cuetos, F., Ellis, A. W., & Alvarez, B. (1999). Naming times for the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures in Spanish. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31, 650-658.
Dell` Acqua, R., Lotto, L., & Job, R. (2000). Naming times and standardized norms for the Italian PD/DPSS set of 266 pictures: Direct comparisons with American, English, French, and Spanish published databases. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 32(588-615).
Dell, G. S. (1986). A Spreading-Activation Theory of Retrieval in Sentence Production. Psychological Review, 93(3), 283-321.
Dell, G. S., Chang, F., & Griffin, Z. M. (1999). Connectionist models of language production: lexical access and grammatical encoding. Cognitive Science, 23(4), 517-542.
Dent, K., Catling, J. C., & Johnston, R. A. (2007). Age of acquisition affects object recognition: Evidence from visual duration thresholds. Acta Psychologica, 125(3), 301-318.
Ellis, A. W., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2000). Age of acquisition effects in adult lexical processing reflect loss of plasticity in maturing systems: Insights from connectionist networks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(5), 1103-1123.
Ellis, A. W., & Morrison, C. M. (1998). Real age-of-acquisition effects in lexical retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(2), 515-523.
Gemba, H., & Sasaki, K. (1989). Potential related to no-go reaction of go/no-go hand movement task with color discrimination in human. Neuroscience Letters, 101, 262-268.
Ghyselinck, M. (2002). Age of acquisition as an organizing variable of the semantic system., Ghent University, Belgium.
Ghyselinck, M., Lewis, M. B., & Brysbaert, M. (2004). Age of acquisition and the cumulative-frequency hypothesis: A review of the literature and a new multi-task investigation. Acta Psychologica, 115(1), 43-67.
Holmes, S. J., & Ellis, A. W. (2006). Age of acquisition and typicality effects in three object processing tasks. Visual Cognition, 13(7/8), 884-910.
Jodo, E., & Kayama, Y. (1992). Relation of a negative ERP component to response inhibition in a go0no-go task. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 82, 477-482.
Johnston, R. A., & Barry, C. (2005). Age of acquisition effects in the semantic processing of pictures. Memory & Cognition, 33(5), 905-912.
Johnston, R. A., & Barry, C. (2006). Age of acquisition and lexical processing. Visual Cognition, 13(7/8), 789-845.
Juhasz, B. J. (2005). Age-of-Acquisition Effects in Word and Picture Identification. Psychological Bulletin, 131(5), 684-712.
Kok, A. (1986). Effects of degradation of visual stimuli on components of the event-related potential (ERP) in go/nogo reaction tasks. BiologicalbPsychology, 23, 21-38.
Kremin, H., Akhutina, T., Basso, A., Davidoff, J., De Wilde, M., Kitzing, P., Lorenz, A., Perrier, D., van der Sandt-Koenderman, M., Vendrel,l J., Weniger, D., Apt, P., Arabia, C., De Bleser, R., Cohen, H., Corbineau, M., Dolivet, M., Hirsh, K., Lehoux, E., Metz-Lutz, M., Montañes, P., Plagne, S., Polonskaya, N., Sirois, M., tachowiak, F., Sweeney, T., Vish-Brink, E. (2003). A cross-linguistic data bank for oral picture naming in Dutch, English, German, French, Italian, Russian, Spanish, and Swedish (PEDOI). Brain And Cognition, 53(2), 243-246.
Kutas, M., & Donchin, E. (1977). The effects of handedness, of responding hand, and of response force on the contralateral dominance of the readiness potential. In J. Desmedt (Ed.), Attention, voluntary contraction, and even-related potentials (pp. 182-210). Basel: Switzerland: Karger.
Kutas, M., & Donchin, E. (1980). Preparation to respond as manifested by movement-related brain potentials. Brain Research, 202, 95-115.
Laws, K. R., Leeson, V. C., & Gale, T. M. (2002). The effect of "masking" on picture naming. Cortex, 38, 137-148.
Levelt, W. J. M., & Roelofs, A. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 22(1), 1.
Liu, K. (2006). Word-Form Encoding in Mandarin Chinese Word Production: The Roles of the Syllable and the Prosodic Frame. Unpublished master`s thesis, National Chuan Cheng university.
Meyer, A. (1991). The time course of phonological encoding in language production: Phonological encoding inside a syllable. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 69-89.
Miller, J., & Hackley, S. A. (1992). Electrophysiological evidence for temporal overlap among contingent mental processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 195-209.
Miller, J., Patterson, T., & Ulrich, R. (1998). Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences. Psychophysiology, 35, 99-115.
Moore, V., Smith-Spark, J. H., & Valentine, T. (2004). The effects of age of acquisition on object recognition. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 417-439.
Morrison, C. M., Ellis, A. W., & Quinlan, P. T. (1992). Age of acquisition, not word frequency, affects object naming, not object recognition. Memory & Cognition, 20(6), 705-714.
Morrison, C. M., & Gibbons, Z. C. (2006). Lexical determinants of semantic processing speed. Visual Cognition, 13(7/8), 949-967.
Morrison, C. M., Hirsh, K. W., & Duggan, G. B. (2003). Age of acquisition, ageing, and verb production: Normative and experimental data. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 56(4), 705.
Osman, A., Bashore, T. R., Coles, M. G. H., Donchin, E., & Meyer, D. E. (1992). On the transmission of partial information: Inferences from movement-related brain potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(1), 217-232.
Peng, Y. (2005). Segments and Features in Word Production by Mandarin Speakers. Unpublished Master`s thesis, National Chuan Cheng university.
Pfefferbaum, A., Ford, J. M., Weller, B. J., & Kopell, B. S. (1985). ERPs to response production and inhibition. . Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 60, 423.
Pind, J., & Tryggvadottir, H. B. (2002). Determinants of picture naming in Icelandic. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43, 221-226.
Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Schmitt, B. M., Kutas, M., & Münte, T. F. (2002). Electrophysiological estimates of the time course of semantic and phonological encoding during listening and naming. Neuropsychologia, 40, 778–787.
Rohrbaugh, J. W. (1984). The orienting reflex: Performance and central nervous system manifestations. In R. P. D. R. Davies (Ed.), Varieties of attention (pp. 324-374). San Diego: CA: Academic Press.
Sasaki, K., & Gemba, H. (1993). Prefrontal cortex in the organization and control of voluntary movement. In T. Ono, L. R. Squire, M. E. Raichle, D. I. Perrett & M. Fukuda (Eds.), Brain mechanisms of perception and memory: From neuron to behavior (pp. 473-496). New York: :Oxford University Press.
Sasaki, K., Gemba, H., Nambu, A., & Matsuzaki, R. (1993). No-go activity in the frontal association cortex of human subjects. Neuroscience Research, 18, 249-252.
Schmitt, B. M., Münte, T., & Kutas, M. (2000). Electrophysiological estimates of the time course of semantic and phonological encoding during implicit picture naming. Psychophysiology, 37, 473–484.
Simson, R., Vaughan, H. G., & Ritter, W. (1977). The scalp topography of potentials in auditory and visual go0nogo tasks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 43, 864-875.
Smid, H. G. O. M., & Mulder, G. (1995). The availability versus the use of partial information: Multiple levels of selective processing between perception and action. Acta Psychologica, 90(1-3), 145-162.
Snodgrass, J. G., & Yuditsky, T. (1996). Naming times for the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28(4), 516-536.
Steyvers, M., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2005). The large-scale structure of semantic networks: Statistical analyses and a model of semantic growth. Cognitive Science, 29, 41–78.
Tomasello, M. (2000). First step toward a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cognitive linguistics, 11(1/2), 61-82.
Van Loon-Vervoorn, W. A. (1989). Eigenschappen van basiswoorden. Lisse: The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Van Turennout, M., Hagoort, P., & Brown, C. M. (1997). Electrophysiological Evidence on the Time Course of Semantic and Phonological Processes in Speech Production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(4), 787-806.
Vaughan, H. G., Costa, L. D., & Ritter, W. (1968). Topography of the human motor potential. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 25, 1-10.
Vitkovitch, M., & Tyrrell, L. (1995). Sources of disagreement in object naming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology/Learning, Memory & Cognition, 48A, 1155-1168.
Wheeldon, L. R., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1995). Monitoring the Time Course of Phonological Encoding. Journal of Memory and Language, 34(3), 311-334.
Zevin, J. D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2002). Age of Acquisition Effects in Word Reading and Other Tasks. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(1), 1-29.
Zhang, Q., & Yang, Y. (2003). The cognitive neuroscience mechanism of speech production. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 35(2), 266-273.
Zhang, Q., & Yang, Y. (2005). The phonological planning unit in Chinese monosyllabic word production. Psychological Science, 28(2), 374-378.
Zhang, Q., & Yang, Y. (2007). Electrophysiological estimates of the time course of semantic and metrical encoding in Chinese speech production. Neuroscience, 147(4), 986-995.
描述 碩士
國立政治大學
語言學研究所
95555014
97
資料來源 http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G0095555014
資料類型 thesis
dc.contributor.advisor 郭文瑞<br>薩文蕙zh_TW
dc.contributor.advisor Kuo, Wen Jui<br>Sah, Wen Huien_US
dc.contributor.author (Authors) 張惠娟zh_TW
dc.contributor.author (Authors) Chang, Hui Chuanen_US
dc.creator (作者) 張惠娟zh_TW
dc.creator (作者) Chang, Hui Chuanen_US
dc.date (日期) 2008en_US
dc.date.accessioned 14-Sep-2009 12:59:45 (UTC+8)-
dc.date.available 14-Sep-2009 12:59:45 (UTC+8)-
dc.date.issued (上傳時間) 14-Sep-2009 12:59:45 (UTC+8)-
dc.identifier (Other Identifiers) G0095555014en_US
dc.identifier.uri (URI) https://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/32046-
dc.description (描述) 碩士zh_TW
dc.description (描述) 國立政治大學zh_TW
dc.description (描述) 語言學研究所zh_TW
dc.description (描述) 95555014zh_TW
dc.description (描述) 97zh_TW
dc.description.abstract (摘要) 習得年齡對語言處理有相當影響,但其成因及作用階段至今仍無定案,根據以知的行為研究成果及假說,結合事件相關電位技術,及雙重選擇按鍵與否典範,我們將嘗試找出習得年齡在言語產出中的作用階段。假定不欲按鍵則無側化動作準備電位,且側化準備電位起始時間取決於,何時取得用以決定反應手的訊息,我們請受試者根據圖片刺激的兩種語言屬性做出正確反應。
      在實驗一中,語意屬性(人造或非人造)決定反應手,圖片名稱第一音節的語音屬性(一、三聲或二、四聲)決定按鍵與否。若習得年齡影響語意處理,我們預期在按鍵情形下,習得年齡早晚兩組的側化準備電位起始時間將有所不同;而不按鍵情形下的側化準備電位持續時間,將反映習得年齡對語音處理的影響。實驗二中,圖片經更嚴格挑選,以控制所有變項,且語言屬性與反應類別間的兩種對應都被測試。
      實驗一和實驗二都發現不按鍵時,若反應手非由語意屬性決定,則側化準備電位不會出現。這個結果符合文獻記載,並暗示語意處理早於語音處理。實驗一中,早習得圖片的唸名反應時間、圖片分類反應時間、及側化準備電位起始時間都較短,支持習得年齡越早,語意提取速度越快。但實驗二中,習得年齡對側化準備電位起始時間沒有顯著影響,儘管前測顯示早習得圖片的唸名反應時間較短,其分類反應時間卻較長,。實驗三進一步肯定,習得年齡對唸名及圖片分類作業有相反效果,以及習得年齡對圖片分類作業的效果不因重複練習而喪失。
      根據完整語音理論 (Brown & Watson, 1987),我們認為當作業要求完整的語音訊息,則習得年齡越早,提取速度越快,唸名作業如是;當作業要求片段的語音訊息,則習得年齡越早,提取速度越慢,額外時間用於解構完整語音訊息,我們的圖片判斷作業如是。實驗二中因少數圖片重複出現,習得年齡對語意處理的正面作用減少,習得年齡對片段語音處理的負面作用卻維持,使得習得年齡組別間,側化準備電位沒有顯著差異,且早習得組圖片分類反應時間反而較長。
      總而言之,本研究支持概念/詞目競爭理論的說法,亦即習得年齡越早,語意提取速度越快。我們的資料也間接反映,習得年齡越早,語音提取速度越慢,但此推論仍需未來研究進一步支持。
zh_TW
dc.description.abstract (摘要) Age-of-acquisition (AoA) is one of the most robust variables for language processing. But its cause and locus during the processing are still under debate. Based on previous behavioral studies and established hypotheses, this study aims to find electrophysiological evidence for the locus of AoA effect during speech production. The continuous measure of speech production is approached by event-related potential (ERP) in conjunction with a manual, dual choice Go/Nogo paradigm. In this paradigm, participants need to choose the correct response according to two linguistic attributes of the target picture. The presumption of this paradigm is that the lateralized readiness potential (LRP), a motor preparation related component, would be absent once the Nogo decision has been made, and its onset marks the time point at which critical information for hand selection is available.
      In Experiment 1, which hand to respond depends on the target picture’s semantic attribute (artifact or non-artifact), to respond or not relies on its phonological attribute (the tone value of its first syllable). It is expected that if AoA influences lexical/semantic processing, LRP onset latency would be different under early and late AoA condition. AoA effect on phonological stage would be reflected on the duration of Nogo LRP. In Experiment 2 the reverse mapping between linguistic attribute and response types was also tested and variables other than AoA were more strictly controlled.
      Both experiments replicate previous finding in that Nogo LRP only occurs when response hand is contingent on semantic information, which indicates that semantic stage precedes phonological stage. In Experiment 1, naming latency, picture classification RT (reaction time), and LRP onset latency were shorter for the early AoA group, indicating accelerated lexical/semantic retrieval. However, in Experiment 2, AoA effect on LRP onset latency was not found. And for items with early AoA, picture classification RT is significantly longer, despite that their naming latency is shorter in the pretest. Experiment 3 further validates the coexistence of the positive AoA effect on naming and negative AoA effect on tone classification and the resistance of the negative AoA effect to repetition.
      Following the phonological completeness hypothesis (Brown & Watson, 1987), we suggest that when the unitary phonological form is demanded, as in the naming task, the early learned words has advantage. When a fraction of the unitary form is demanded, as in the tone classification task, extra time is needed to decompose the retrieved unitary form. The positive AoA effect on semantic stage is larger in Experiment 1 due to material selection and number of repetition. Thus in Experiment 1, the positive AoA effect on semantic stage remains, but in Experiment 2, it is overpowered by the negative AoA effect on fractional phonological extraction, resulting in the absence of AoA effect on LRP pattern and longer picture classification RT under early AoA condition.
      In sum, results of this study indicate that semantic feature can be retrieved faster under early AoA condition, which is in line with concept/lemma competition hypothesis. Our results also imply slower tone retrieval under early AoA condition, which need to be further testified by future studies.
en_US
dc.description.tableofcontents Acknowledgements………………………………………………………iv
     Table of Contents………………………………………………………v
     Chinese Abstract……………………………………………………vii
     English Abstract………………………………………………………ix
     Chapter
     1. Introduction…………………………………………………………1
     2. Literature Review…………………………………………………5
     2.1 Modeling speech production…………………………………5
     2.2 Age-of-acquisition effect…………………………………8
     2.2.1 Empirical findings of AoA effect………………………8
     2.2.2 Theories of AoA effect……………………………………12
     2.3 Behavioral studies in production of Mandarin Chinese…16
     2.3.1 Experimental paradigm………………………………………16
     2.3.2 Previous studies in Mandarin Chinese………………19
     2.4 Electrophysiological studies in speech production…21
     2.4.1 Self-monitoring…………………………………………………21
     2.4.2 Lateralized readiness potential…………………………22
     2.4.3 Previous electrophysiological studies in speech production studies…………………………………………………24
     3 Research purpose…………………………………………………36
     4. Experiment…………………………………………………………38
     4.1 Pretests…………………………………………………………38
     4.2 Experiment 1…………………………………………………………41
     4.2.1 Participants and materials……………………………………41
     4.2.2 Design and procedure…………………………………………42
     4.2.3 ERPs recordings and data analysis…………………………44
     4.2.4 Results…………………………………………………………46
     4.2.5 Discussion………………………………………………………48
     4.3 Experiment 2…………………………………………………………50
     4.3.1 Participants and materials………………………………51
     4.3.2 Design and procedure…………………………………………52
     4.3.3 ERPs recordings and data analysis…………………………53
     4.3.4 Results…………………………………………………………55
     4.3.5 Discussion………………………………………………………63
     4.4 Experiment 3…………………………………………………………64
     4.4.1 Participants and materials……………………………………64
     4.4.2 Design and procedure…………………………………………65
     4.4.3 Results…………………………………………………………66
     4.4.4 Discussion………………………………………………………67
     5. General Discussion………………………………………………………69
     References……………………………………………………………………73
     Appendixes
     A. Test paper used in the rating pretest…………………………81
     B. Stimulus of Experiment 1……………………………………………82
     C. Stimulus of Experiment 2……………………………………………84
zh_TW
dc.language.iso en_US-
dc.source.uri (資料來源) http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G0095555014en_US
dc.subject (關鍵詞) 事件相關電位zh_TW
dc.subject (關鍵詞) 言語產製zh_TW
dc.subject (關鍵詞) 習得年齡zh_TW
dc.subject (關鍵詞) 側化準備電位zh_TW
dc.subject (關鍵詞) Eevent-related potentialen_US
dc.subject (關鍵詞) Speech productionen_US
dc.subject (關鍵詞) Age-of-Acquisitionen_US
dc.subject (關鍵詞) Lateral readiness potentialen_US
dc.title (題名) 中文言語產製中語意及語音處理的電生理訊號zh_TW
dc.title (題名) Electrophysiological signatures of semantic and phonological processing in mandarin Chinese speech productionen_US
dc.type (資料類型) thesisen
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Abdel Rahman, R., & Sommer, W. (2003). Does phonological encoding in speech production always follow the retrieval of semantic knowledge?: Electrophysiological evidence for parallel processing. Cognitive Brain Research, 16(3), 372-382.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Abdel Rahman, R., Van Turennout, M., & Levelt, W. J. M. (2003). Phonological Encoding Is Not Contingent on Semantic Feature Retrieval: An Electrophysiological Study on Object Naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(5), 850-860.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Barry, C., Morrison, C. M., & Ellis, A. W. (1997). Naming the Snodgrass and Vanderwart Pictures: Effects of Age of Acquisition, Frequency, and Name Agreement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 50(3), 560-585.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Belke, E., Brysbaert, M., Meyer, A. S., & Ghyselinck, M. (2005). Age of acquisition effects in picture naming: evidence for a lexical-semantic competition hypothesis. Cognition, 96(2), B45-B54.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Bonin, P., Chalard, M., Meot, A., & Barry, C. (2006). Are age-of-acquisition effects on object naming due simply to difference in object recognition? comments on Levelt (2002) Memory & cognition, 34(5), 1172-1182.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Bonin, P., Chalard, M., Meot, A., & Fayol, M. (2002). The determinants of spoken and written picture naming latencies. British Journal of Psychology, 93(1), 89.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Bonin, P., Fayol, M., & Chalard, M. (2001). Age of acquisition and word frequency in written picture naming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 54(2), 469-489.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Brown, G. D., & Watson, F. L. (1987). First in, first out: word learning age and spoken word frequency as predictors of word familiarity and word naming latency. Memory & Cognition, 15(3), 208-216.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Brysbaert, M., & Ghyselinck, M. (2006). The effect of age of acquisition: Partly frequency related, partly frequency independent. Visual Cognition, 13(7/8), 992-1011.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Brysbaert, M., Van Wijnendaele, I., & De Deyne, S. (2000). Age-of-acquisition effects in semantic processing tasks. Acta Psychologica, 104(2), 215-226.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Carroll, J. B., & White, M. N. (1973). Word frequency and age-of-acquisition as determiners of picture-naming latency. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 85–95.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Catling, J. C., & Johnston, R. A. (2006). The effects of age of acquisition on an object classification task. Visual Cognition, 13(7/8), 968-980.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Chalard, M., & Bonin, P. (2006). Age-of-acquisition effects in picture naming: Are they structural and/or semantic in nature? Visual Cognition, 13(7/8), 864-883.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Chen, J.-Y., Chen, T.-M., & Dell, G. S. (2002). Word-form encoding in Mandarin Chinese as assessed by the implicit priming task. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(4), 751-781.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Chen, J.-Y., Wei-Chun, L., & Ludovic, F. (2003). Masked priming of the syllable in Chinese speech production. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 45(1), 107-120.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Cuetos, F., Ellis, A. W., & Alvarez, B. (1999). Naming times for the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures in Spanish. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31, 650-658.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Dell` Acqua, R., Lotto, L., & Job, R. (2000). Naming times and standardized norms for the Italian PD/DPSS set of 266 pictures: Direct comparisons with American, English, French, and Spanish published databases. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 32(588-615).zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Dell, G. S. (1986). A Spreading-Activation Theory of Retrieval in Sentence Production. Psychological Review, 93(3), 283-321.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Dell, G. S., Chang, F., & Griffin, Z. M. (1999). Connectionist models of language production: lexical access and grammatical encoding. Cognitive Science, 23(4), 517-542.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Dent, K., Catling, J. C., & Johnston, R. A. (2007). Age of acquisition affects object recognition: Evidence from visual duration thresholds. Acta Psychologica, 125(3), 301-318.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Ellis, A. W., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2000). Age of acquisition effects in adult lexical processing reflect loss of plasticity in maturing systems: Insights from connectionist networks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(5), 1103-1123.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Ellis, A. W., & Morrison, C. M. (1998). Real age-of-acquisition effects in lexical retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(2), 515-523.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Gemba, H., & Sasaki, K. (1989). Potential related to no-go reaction of go/no-go hand movement task with color discrimination in human. Neuroscience Letters, 101, 262-268.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Ghyselinck, M. (2002). Age of acquisition as an organizing variable of the semantic system., Ghent University, Belgium.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Ghyselinck, M., Lewis, M. B., & Brysbaert, M. (2004). Age of acquisition and the cumulative-frequency hypothesis: A review of the literature and a new multi-task investigation. Acta Psychologica, 115(1), 43-67.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Holmes, S. J., & Ellis, A. W. (2006). Age of acquisition and typicality effects in three object processing tasks. Visual Cognition, 13(7/8), 884-910.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Jodo, E., & Kayama, Y. (1992). Relation of a negative ERP component to response inhibition in a go0no-go task. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 82, 477-482.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Johnston, R. A., & Barry, C. (2005). Age of acquisition effects in the semantic processing of pictures. Memory & Cognition, 33(5), 905-912.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Johnston, R. A., & Barry, C. (2006). Age of acquisition and lexical processing. Visual Cognition, 13(7/8), 789-845.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Juhasz, B. J. (2005). Age-of-Acquisition Effects in Word and Picture Identification. Psychological Bulletin, 131(5), 684-712.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Kok, A. (1986). Effects of degradation of visual stimuli on components of the event-related potential (ERP) in go/nogo reaction tasks. BiologicalbPsychology, 23, 21-38.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Kremin, H., Akhutina, T., Basso, A., Davidoff, J., De Wilde, M., Kitzing, P., Lorenz, A., Perrier, D., van der Sandt-Koenderman, M., Vendrel,l J., Weniger, D., Apt, P., Arabia, C., De Bleser, R., Cohen, H., Corbineau, M., Dolivet, M., Hirsh, K., Lehoux, E., Metz-Lutz, M., Montañes, P., Plagne, S., Polonskaya, N., Sirois, M., tachowiak, F., Sweeney, T., Vish-Brink, E. (2003). A cross-linguistic data bank for oral picture naming in Dutch, English, German, French, Italian, Russian, Spanish, and Swedish (PEDOI). Brain And Cognition, 53(2), 243-246.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Kutas, M., & Donchin, E. (1977). The effects of handedness, of responding hand, and of response force on the contralateral dominance of the readiness potential. In J. Desmedt (Ed.), Attention, voluntary contraction, and even-related potentials (pp. 182-210). Basel: Switzerland: Karger.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Kutas, M., & Donchin, E. (1980). Preparation to respond as manifested by movement-related brain potentials. Brain Research, 202, 95-115.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Laws, K. R., Leeson, V. C., & Gale, T. M. (2002). The effect of "masking" on picture naming. Cortex, 38, 137-148.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Levelt, W. J. M., & Roelofs, A. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 22(1), 1.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Liu, K. (2006). Word-Form Encoding in Mandarin Chinese Word Production: The Roles of the Syllable and the Prosodic Frame. Unpublished master`s thesis, National Chuan Cheng university.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Meyer, A. (1991). The time course of phonological encoding in language production: Phonological encoding inside a syllable. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 69-89.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Miller, J., & Hackley, S. A. (1992). Electrophysiological evidence for temporal overlap among contingent mental processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 195-209.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Miller, J., Patterson, T., & Ulrich, R. (1998). Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences. Psychophysiology, 35, 99-115.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Moore, V., Smith-Spark, J. H., & Valentine, T. (2004). The effects of age of acquisition on object recognition. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 417-439.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Morrison, C. M., Ellis, A. W., & Quinlan, P. T. (1992). Age of acquisition, not word frequency, affects object naming, not object recognition. Memory & Cognition, 20(6), 705-714.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Morrison, C. M., & Gibbons, Z. C. (2006). Lexical determinants of semantic processing speed. Visual Cognition, 13(7/8), 949-967.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Morrison, C. M., Hirsh, K. W., & Duggan, G. B. (2003). Age of acquisition, ageing, and verb production: Normative and experimental data. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 56(4), 705.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Osman, A., Bashore, T. R., Coles, M. G. H., Donchin, E., & Meyer, D. E. (1992). On the transmission of partial information: Inferences from movement-related brain potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(1), 217-232.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Peng, Y. (2005). Segments and Features in Word Production by Mandarin Speakers. Unpublished Master`s thesis, National Chuan Cheng university.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Pfefferbaum, A., Ford, J. M., Weller, B. J., & Kopell, B. S. (1985). ERPs to response production and inhibition. . Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 60, 423.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Pind, J., & Tryggvadottir, H. B. (2002). Determinants of picture naming in Icelandic. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43, 221-226.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Schmitt, B. M., Kutas, M., & Münte, T. F. (2002). Electrophysiological estimates of the time course of semantic and phonological encoding during listening and naming. Neuropsychologia, 40, 778–787.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Rohrbaugh, J. W. (1984). The orienting reflex: Performance and central nervous system manifestations. In R. P. D. R. Davies (Ed.), Varieties of attention (pp. 324-374). San Diego: CA: Academic Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Sasaki, K., & Gemba, H. (1993). Prefrontal cortex in the organization and control of voluntary movement. In T. Ono, L. R. Squire, M. E. Raichle, D. I. Perrett & M. Fukuda (Eds.), Brain mechanisms of perception and memory: From neuron to behavior (pp. 473-496). New York: :Oxford University Press.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Sasaki, K., Gemba, H., Nambu, A., & Matsuzaki, R. (1993). No-go activity in the frontal association cortex of human subjects. Neuroscience Research, 18, 249-252.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Schmitt, B. M., Münte, T., & Kutas, M. (2000). Electrophysiological estimates of the time course of semantic and phonological encoding during implicit picture naming. Psychophysiology, 37, 473–484.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Simson, R., Vaughan, H. G., & Ritter, W. (1977). The scalp topography of potentials in auditory and visual go0nogo tasks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 43, 864-875.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Smid, H. G. O. M., & Mulder, G. (1995). The availability versus the use of partial information: Multiple levels of selective processing between perception and action. Acta Psychologica, 90(1-3), 145-162.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Snodgrass, J. G., & Yuditsky, T. (1996). Naming times for the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28(4), 516-536.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Steyvers, M., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2005). The large-scale structure of semantic networks: Statistical analyses and a model of semantic growth. Cognitive Science, 29, 41–78.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Tomasello, M. (2000). First step toward a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cognitive linguistics, 11(1/2), 61-82.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Van Loon-Vervoorn, W. A. (1989). Eigenschappen van basiswoorden. Lisse: The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Van Turennout, M., Hagoort, P., & Brown, C. M. (1997). Electrophysiological Evidence on the Time Course of Semantic and Phonological Processes in Speech Production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(4), 787-806.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Vaughan, H. G., Costa, L. D., & Ritter, W. (1968). Topography of the human motor potential. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 25, 1-10.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Vitkovitch, M., & Tyrrell, L. (1995). Sources of disagreement in object naming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology/Learning, Memory & Cognition, 48A, 1155-1168.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Wheeldon, L. R., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1995). Monitoring the Time Course of Phonological Encoding. Journal of Memory and Language, 34(3), 311-334.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Zevin, J. D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2002). Age of Acquisition Effects in Word Reading and Other Tasks. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(1), 1-29.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Zhang, Q., & Yang, Y. (2003). The cognitive neuroscience mechanism of speech production. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 35(2), 266-273.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Zhang, Q., & Yang, Y. (2005). The phonological planning unit in Chinese monosyllabic word production. Psychological Science, 28(2), 374-378.zh_TW
dc.relation.reference (參考文獻) Zhang, Q., & Yang, Y. (2007). Electrophysiological estimates of the time course of semantic and metrical encoding in Chinese speech production. Neuroscience, 147(4), 986-995.zh_TW